Impact of attentional focus on motor performance within the context of "early" limb regulation and "late" target control.

Directing attention to the effect of one's movement (external focus) has been shown to aid performance compared to directing attention to the movement itself (internal focus). This finding has been predominantly explained by an external focus promoting action planning and automatic movement control, while an internal focus acts to constrain movement (constrained action hypothesis [CAH]). In a separate line of research, the multiple control process model states that early movement phases involve anticipated and feedforward processes, while late movement phases explicitly incorporate external afferent information. We hypothesized that enhanced planning and automatic movement control would manifest from an external/distal focus compared to internal/proximal focus. The present study had participants execute fast and accurate movements to a single target using a digitizing graphics tablet that translated movements to a screen. Participants were instructed to focus on the end target location (external-distal), movement of the cursor (external-proximal), and movement of the limb (internal-proximal). It was found that the external-distal focus generated a shorter time to initiate and execute movements (indicating enhanced movement planning) compared to the external- and internal-proximal conditions. In addition, only the external proximal focus revealed a reduction in spatial variability between peak velocity and movement end (indicating greater online control). These findings indicate that advances in action planning and online control occur when adopting an external-distal focus. However, there were some benefits to online control when adopting an external-proximal focus. We propose that an external-distal focus promotes action-effect principles, where there is a greater contribution of anticipatory feedforward processes that limit the need for late online control.

[1]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the control of movement amplitude. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[2]  Moving with an external focus: Automatic or simply less demanding. , 2007 .

[3]  C. Heyes Causes and consequences of imitation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  W. Prinz,et al.  Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: A review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  W. Helsen,et al.  Temporal and spatial coupling of point of gaze and hand movements in aiming. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  A. Healy,et al.  How changing the focus of attention affects performance, kinematics, and electromyography in dart throwing. , 2010, Human movement science.

[8]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[9]  R. Gray,et al.  Effects of focus of attention on baseball batting performance in players of differing skill levels. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[10]  C. Dugas,et al.  On the type of information used to control and learn an aiming movement after moderate and extensive training , 1987 .

[11]  Romeo Chua,et al.  No automatic pilot for visually guided aiming based on colour , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  S. Glover,et al.  The Motor-Cognitive Model of Motor Imagery: Evidence From Timing Errors in Simulated Reaching and Grasping , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  Frédéric Crevecoeur,et al.  A perspective on multisensory integration and rapid perturbation responses , 2015, Vision Research.

[14]  M. Seccareccia,et al.  Reciprocal Influences , 2013, Roman North Africa.

[15]  G. Wulf,et al.  Increased movement accuracy and reduced EMG activity as the result of adopting an external focus of attention , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.

[16]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Understanding the role of augmented feedback : The good, the bad and the ugly , 2004 .

[17]  G. Wulf,et al.  An external focus of attention attenuates balance impairment in patients with Parkinson's disease who have a fall history , 2005 .

[18]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[19]  G. Wulf,et al.  An External Focus of Attention Enhances Golf Shot Accuracy in Beginners and Experts , 2007, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[20]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[21]  Matt Greig,et al.  Attentional Focusing Instructions Influence Force Production and Muscular Activity During Isokinetic Elbow Flexions , 2009, Journal of strength and conditioning research.

[22]  D. Marchant,et al.  Instructions to Adopt an External Focus Enhance Muscular Endurance , 2011, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[23]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[24]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic coordination in the brain , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  M. Crawshaw,et al.  The effects of attentional focusing strategies on novice dart throwing performance and Their task experiences , 2007 .

[26]  G. Wulf,et al.  EMG Activity as a Function of the Performer's Focus of Attention , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  Gabriele Wulf,et al.  Reciprocal Influences of Attentional Focus on Postural and Suprapostural Task Performance , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Are there age-related differences in learning to optimize speed, accuracy, and energy expenditure? , 2007, Human movement science.

[29]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory , 1999 .

[30]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Evidence for Continuous Processing of Visual Information in a Manual Video-Aiming Task , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  G. J. Thomas The Co-ordination and Regulation of Movements , 1967 .

[32]  C. Shea,et al.  The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[33]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  Abdelkader Gouaïch,et al.  Trajectory formation principles are the same after mild or moderate stroke , 2017, PloS one.

[35]  B. Hommel,et al.  Effect anticipation and action control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  Michael A. Khan,et al.  The Effect of Practice on the Control of Rapid Aiming Movements: Evidence for an Interdependency between Programming and Feedback Processing , 1998 .

[37]  James L. Lyons,et al.  The multiple process model of goal-directed reaching revisited , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[38]  Increases in Jump-and-Reach Height through an External Focus of Attention , 2007 .

[39]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Preserved Adjustment but Impaired Awareness in a Sensory-Motor Conflict following Prefrontal Lesions , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  R. Masters Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure , 1992 .

[41]  Lawrence E. M. Grierson,et al.  Kinematic analysis of goal-directed aims made against early and late perturbations: an investigation of the relative influence of two online control processes. , 2008, Human movement science.

[42]  Gabriele Wulf,et al.  The Influence of External and Internal Foci of Attention on Transfer to Novel Situations and Skills , 2003, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[43]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  Differences in visuomotor control between the upper and lower visual fields , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  Leonie Oostwoud Wijdenes,et al.  Movement Adjustments Have Short Latencies Because There is No Need to Detect Anything. , 2016, Motor control.

[45]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Common vs. independent limb control in sequential vertical aiming: The cost of potential errors during extensions and reversals. , 2016, Acta psychologica.

[47]  J. Hardy,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Journal of Applied Sport Psychology Effects of Attentional Focus on Skilled Performance in Golf , 2022 .

[48]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  The Movement Kinematics and Learning Strategies Associated with Adopting Different Foci of Attention during Both Acquisition and Anxious Performance , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[49]  Howard N. Zelaznik Practice does not lead to changes in movement trajectories in Fitts' Law tasks , 2018 .

[50]  Luc Tremblay,et al.  Learning to Optimize Speed, Accuracy, and Energy Expenditure: A Framework for Understanding Speed-Accuracy Relations in Goal-Directed Aiming , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[51]  G. Wulf,et al.  Enhancing the Learning of Sport Skills Through External-Focus Feedback , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[52]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  Inferring online and offline processing of visual feedback in target-directed movements from kinematic data , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[53]  G. Wulf Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years , 2013 .

[54]  Uta Sailer 차량 내부 인터페이스 기반 운전자의 Eye-Hand Coordination 과 운전 부하간의 관계 모델 , 2016 .

[55]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Programming strategies for rapid aiming movements under simple and choice reaction time conditions , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[56]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[57]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[58]  J. Burridge,et al.  Internal and External Focus of Attention During Gait Re-Education: An Observational Study of Physical Therapist Practice in Stroke Rehabilitation , 2013, Physical Therapy.

[59]  C. Shea,et al.  Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances learning , 2003, Psychological research.

[60]  S. Fasoli,et al.  Effect of instructions on functional reach in persons with and without cerebrovascular accident. , 2002, The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association.

[61]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[62]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[63]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[64]  D. C. Shapiro,et al.  Effects of a secondary task on the accuracy of single aiming movements. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  W. Helsen,et al.  Eye-Hand Coordination in Goal-Directed Action: Normal and Pathological Functioning , 2010 .

[66]  Steven R Passmore,et al.  Location versus task relevance: The impact of differing internal focus of attention instructions on motor performance. , 2017, Acta psychologica.

[67]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  The contribution of peripheral and central vision in the control of movement amplitude. , 2006, Human movement science.

[68]  J Paillard,et al.  Fast and slow feedback loops for the visual correction of spatial errors in a pointing task: a reappraisal. , 1996, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.