Is functional integration of resting state brain networks an unspecific biomarker for working memory performance?

Is there one optimal topology of functional brain networks at rest from which our cognitive performance would profit? Previous studies suggest that functional integration of resting state brain networks is an important biomarker for cognitive performance. However, it is still unknown whether higher network integration is an unspecific predictor for good cognitive performance or, alternatively, whether specific network organization during rest predicts only specific cognitive abilities. Here, we investigated the relationship between network integration at rest and cognitive performance using two tasks that measured different aspects of working memory; one task assessed visual-spatial and the other numerical working memory. Network clustering, modularity and efficiency were computed to capture network integration on different levels of network organization, and to statistically compare their correlations with the performance in each working memory test. The results revealed that each working memory aspect profits from a different resting state topology, and the tests showed significantly different correlations with each of the measures of network integration. While higher global network integration and modularity predicted significantly better performance in visual-spatial working memory, both measures showed no significant correlation with numerical working memory performance. In contrast, numerical working memory was superior in subjects with highly clustered brain networks, predominantly in the intraparietal sulcus, a core brain region of the working memory network. Our findings suggest that a specific balance between local and global functional integration of resting state brain networks facilitates special aspects of cognitive performance. In the context of working memory, while visual-spatial performance is facilitated by globally integrated functional resting state brain networks, numerical working memory profits from increased capacities for local processing, especially in brain regions involved in working memory performance.

[1]  S. Rombouts,et al.  Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience Systems Neuroscience , 2022 .

[2]  L. Cronbach Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests , 1951 .

[3]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Dissociation of Storage and Rehearsal in Verbal Working Memory: Evidence From Positron Emission Tomography , 1996 .

[4]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Working Memory: A View from Neuroimaging , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Heinz Holling,et al.  The measurement of visuo–spatial and verbal–numerical working memory: Development of IRT-based scales , 2008 .

[6]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Reproducibility of graph metrics of human brain functional networks , 2009, NeuroImage.

[7]  B. Baune,et al.  Exploring Visual–Spatial Working Memory: A Critical Review of Concepts and Models , 2009, Neuropsychology Review.

[8]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Resting-state fMRI in the Human Connectome Project , 2013, NeuroImage.

[9]  J. Myerson,et al.  Selective interference with the maintenance of location information in working memory. , 1996 .

[10]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of working memory: , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  R. Kahn,et al.  Efficiency of Functional Brain Networks and Intellectual Performance , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  C. Spearman The proof and measurement of association between two things. , 2015, International journal of epidemiology.

[13]  Gerhard Werner,et al.  Fractals in the Nervous System: Conceptual Implications for Theoretical Neuroscience , 2009, Front. Physiology.

[14]  Jean-Loup Guillaume,et al.  Fast unfolding of communities in large networks , 2008, 0803.0476.

[15]  Michael F. Bunting,et al.  Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations , 2010, NeuroImage.

[17]  Iring Koch,et al.  The role of crosstalk in dual-task performance: evidence from manipulating response-code overlap , 2009, Psychological research.

[18]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  A. Baddeley Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. , 2012, Annual review of psychology.

[20]  Uri Alon,et al.  Varying environments can speed up evolution , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  SYSTEMS NEUROSCIENCE Original Research Article , 2009 .

[22]  John Suckling,et al.  A wavelet method for modeling and despiking motion artifacts from resting-state fMRI time series , 2014, NeuroImage.

[23]  Hidenao Fukuyama,et al.  Functional roles of the cingulo-frontal network in performance on working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[24]  L. Jäncke,et al.  The effects of working memory training on functional brain network efficiency , 2013, Cortex.

[25]  Susan D. Voyer,et al.  Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  A. Wagner,et al.  Robustness and evolution: concepts, insights and challenges from a developmental model system , 2008, Heredity.

[27]  Archana Venkataraman,et al.  Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Modular and Hierarchically Modular Organization of Brain Networks , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[29]  Kathryn M. McMillan,et al.  N‐back working memory paradigm: A meta‐analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[30]  Enzo Tagliazucchi,et al.  Automatic sleep staging using fMRI functional connectivity data , 2012, NeuroImage.

[31]  M. Greenlee,et al.  Behavioral and Brain Functions , 2007 .

[32]  Rex E. Jung,et al.  Distributed brain sites for the g-factor of intelligence , 2006, NeuroImage.

[33]  Klaus Lehnertz,et al.  Incidental and Intentional Learning of Verbal Episodic Material Differentially Modifies Functional Brain Networks , 2013, PloS one.

[34]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Functional Connectivity and Brain Networks in Schizophrenia , 2010 .

[35]  R. Engle,et al.  The mechanisms of working memory capacity: Primary memory, secondary memory, and attention control , 2014 .

[36]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Brain Network Analysis: Separating Cost from Topology Using Cost-Integration , 2011, PloS one.

[37]  D. Fair,et al.  Hemispheric lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory during adolescence , 2013, Brain and Cognition.

[38]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Hierarchical Organization of Human Cortical Networks in Health and Schizophrenia , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Christina B. Young,et al.  Functional dissociations between four basic arithmetic operations in the human posterior parietal cortex: A cytoarchitectonic mapping study , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  N. C. Silver,et al.  A Monte Carlo Evaluation of Tests for Comparing Dependent Correlations , 2003, The Journal of general psychology.

[42]  R. Logie The Functional Organization and Capacity Limits of Working Memory , 2011 .

[43]  D. Hubel,et al.  The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  M E J Newman,et al.  Modularity and community structure in networks. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  D Purves,et al.  The extraordinarily rapid disappearance of entoptic images. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  T. Pansell,et al.  Slow oscillatory eye movement during visual fixation , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Malik Magdon-Ismail,et al.  Finding communities by clustering a graph into overlapping subgraphs , 2005, IADIS AC.

[48]  P. Ackerman,et al.  Working Memory and Intelligence : The Same or Different Constructs ? , 2005 .

[49]  Hod Lipson,et al.  The evolutionary origins of modularity , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[50]  L. Henry,et al.  The Development of Working Memory in Children , 2011 .

[51]  R. Engle,et al.  The generality of working memory capacity: a latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[52]  Kim F. Nimon,et al.  The Assumption of a Reliable Instrument and Other Pitfalls to Avoid When Considering the Reliability of Data , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[53]  P Riley,et al.  Dynamical reconnection and stability constraints on cortical network architecture. , 2009, Physical review letters.

[54]  S Hale,et al.  Selective interference with verbal and spatial working memory in young and older adults. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[55]  Matthew J. Brookes,et al.  Understanding gradient artefacts in simultaneous EEG/fMRI , 2009, NeuroImage.

[56]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Network attributes for segregation and integration in the human brain , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[57]  O. Sporns,et al.  The economy of brain network organization , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[58]  Alina Nazareth,et al.  Explaining sex differences in mental rotation: role of spatial activity experience , 2013, Cognitive Processing.

[59]  L. Jiao,et al.  Adjusting from disjoint to overlapping community detection of complex networks , 2009 .

[60]  Hidenao Fukuyama,et al.  The neural basis of executive function in working memory: an fMRI study based on individual differences , 2004, NeuroImage.

[61]  Jacobus F. A. Jansen,et al.  Functional and Structural Network Impairment in Childhood Frontal Lobe Epilepsy , 2014, PloS one.

[62]  D. Allen,et al.  Presentation modality influences WAIS Digit Span performance in younger and older adults , 2008, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[63]  Manfred G Kitzbichler,et al.  Cognitive Effort Drives Workspace Configuration of Human Brain Functional Networks , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  A. Stevens,et al.  Functional Brain Network Modularity Captures Inter- and Intra-Individual Variation in Working Memory Capacity , 2012, PloS one.

[65]  K. Hasan,et al.  Working memory load modulation of parieto‐frontal connections: Evidence from dynamic causal modeling , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[66]  Ameera X. Patel,et al.  Long-Term Effects of Attentional Performance on Functional Brain Network Topology , 2013, PloS one.

[67]  H. Laufs,et al.  Decoding Wakefulness Levels from Typical fMRI Resting-State Data Reveals Reliable Drifts between Wakefulness and Sleep , 2014, Neuron.

[68]  O. J. Dunn,et al.  Correlation Coefficients Measured on the Same Individuals , 1969 .

[69]  U. Alon,et al.  Spontaneous evolution of modularity and network motifs. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[70]  Duncan J. Watts,et al.  Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks , 1998, Nature.

[71]  R. Haier,et al.  The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[72]  J. Matias Palva,et al.  Graph properties of synchronized cortical networks during visual working memory maintenance , 2010, NeuroImage.

[73]  U. Ziemann,et al.  Working memory performance of early MS patients correlates inversely with modularity increases in resting state functional connectivity networks , 2014, NeuroImage.

[74]  Simon Farrell,et al.  Modeling working memory: An interference model of complex span , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[75]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[76]  M. Barnes,et al.  Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. , 2010 .

[77]  Ralf Schulze,et al.  Working-memory capacity explains reasoning abilityand a little bit more , 2002 .

[78]  V Latora,et al.  Efficient behavior of small-world networks. , 2001, Physical review letters.

[79]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Brain Connectivity Related to Working Memory Performance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[80]  Danielle S Bassett,et al.  Cognitive fitness of cost-efficient brain functional networks , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[81]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Task Decomposition Through Competition in a Modular Connectionist Architecture: The What and Where Vision Tasks , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[82]  C. Montag,et al.  Assessing the function of the fronto‐parietal attention network: Insights from resting‐state fMRI and the attentional network test , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[83]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis , 2012, NeuroImage.

[84]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Human Brain Functional Network Changes Associated with Enhanced and Impaired Attentional Task Performance , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[85]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion , 2012, NeuroImage.

[86]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[87]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Whole-brain anatomical networks: Does the choice of nodes matter? , 2010, NeuroImage.

[88]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience Systems Neuroscience , 2022 .

[89]  E. Bullmore,et al.  A Resilient, Low-Frequency, Small-World Human Brain Functional Network with Highly Connected Association Cortical Hubs , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[90]  O. Wilhelm,et al.  Working memory capacity - facets of a cognitive ability construct , 2000 .

[91]  Michael R. Chernick,et al.  Wavelet Methods for Time Series Analysis , 2001, Technometrics.

[92]  Patrick Dupont,et al.  Motor learning-induced changes in functional brain connectivity as revealed by means of graph-theoretical network analysis , 2012, NeuroImage.

[93]  R Cameron Craddock,et al.  A whole brain fMRI atlas generated via spatially constrained spectral clustering , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[94]  P. Barrouillet,et al.  On the law relating processing to storage in working memory. , 2011, Psychological review.

[95]  W. Maier,et al.  Inferior frontal gyrus preserves working memory and emotional learning under conditions of impaired noradrenergic signaling , 2013, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[96]  Jun Li,et al.  Brain Anatomical Network and Intelligence , 2009, NeuroImage.

[97]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Interference between storage and processing in working memory: Feature overwriting, not similarity-based competition , 2009, Memory & cognition.

[98]  E. Åhsberg,et al.  Perceived quality of fatigue during different occupational tasks Development of a questionnaire , 1997 .

[99]  M. Linn,et al.  Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. , 1985, Child development.

[100]  M. Hallett Human Brain Function , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.