Principles of the Guidance of Exploration for Orientation and Specification of Action

To control movement of any type, the neural system requires perceptual information to distinguish what actions are possible in any given environment. The behavior aimed at collecting this information, termed “exploration”, is vital for successful movement control. Currently, the main function of exploration is understood in the context of specifying the requirements of the task at hand. To accommodate for agency and action-selection, we propose that this understanding needs to be supplemented with a function of exploration that logically precedes the specification of action requirements with the purpose of discovery of possibilities for action—action orientation. This study aimed to provide evidence for the delineation of exploration for action orientation and exploration for action specification using the principles from “General Tau Theory.” Sixteen male participants volunteered and performed a laboratory-based exploration task. The visual scenes of different task-specific situations were projected on five monitors surrounding the participant. At a predetermined time, the participant received a simulated ball and was asked to respond by indicating where they would next play the ball. Head movements were recorded using inertial sensors as a measure of exploratory activity. It was shown that movement guidance characteristics varied between different head turns as participants moved from exploration for orientation to exploration for action specification. The first head turn in the trial, used for action-orientation, showed later peaks in the velocity profile and harder closure of the movement gap (gap between the start and end of the head-movement) in comparison to the later head turns. However, no differences were found between the first and the final head turn, which we hypothesized are used mainly for action orientation and specification respectively. These results are in support of differences in the function and control of head movement for discovery of opportunities for action (orientation) vs. head movement for specification of task requirements. Both are important for natural movement, yet in experimental settings,orientation is often neglected. Including both orientation and action specification in an experimental design should maximize generalizability of an experiment to natural behavior. Future studies are required to study the neural bases of movement guidance in order to better understand exploration in anticipation of movement.

[1]  E. Reed Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. , 1997 .

[2]  David N. Lee,et al.  TauG-guidance of transients in expressive musical performance , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  M. Hollands,et al.  Can telling older adults where to look reduce falls? Evidence for a causal link between inappropriate visual sampling and suboptimal stepping performance , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  R. McNeill Alexander,et al.  Principles of Animal Locomotion , 2002 .

[5]  A. V. D. van der Meer,et al.  TauG-guidance of dynamic balance control during gait initiation in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome and fibromyalgia. , 2016, Clinical biomechanics.

[6]  D. Sparks,et al.  Eye-head coordination during head-unrestrained gaze shifts in rhesus monkeys. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Joan N. Vickers,et al.  Origins and current issues in Quiet Eye research , 2016 .

[8]  Kip Smith,et al.  Situation Awareness Is Adaptive, Externally Directed Consciousness , 1995, Hum. Factors.

[9]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[10]  David N. Lee,et al.  A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.

[11]  Perceptually Guided Action: A Step in the Right Direction , 2008, Current Biology.

[12]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Exploration in the service of prospective control , 2000 .

[13]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[14]  J. Gibson Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[15]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Visual control of foot placement when walking over rough terrain , 2011 .

[16]  M. Cole,et al.  Using Microtechnology to Quantify Torso Angle During Match-Play in Field Hockey. , 2019, Journal of strength and conditioning research.

[17]  David N. Lee,et al.  Prospective guidance in a free-swimming cell , 2012, Biological Cybernetics.

[18]  K. Hadjidimitrakis,et al.  Eye position modulates the electromyographic responses of neck muscles to electrical stimulation of the superior colliculus in the alert cat , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  M. Cole,et al.  Influence of age and falls incidence on tau guidance of centre of pressure movement during gait initiation. , 2019, Gait & posture.

[20]  Michael H. Cole,et al.  Don’t Turn Blind! The Relationship Between Exploration Before Ball Possession and On-Ball Performance in Association Football , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[21]  G. Pepping,et al.  Effects of Affordance Perception on the Initiation and Actualization of Action , 2010 .

[22]  Lea Fleischer,et al.  The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 2016 .

[23]  Thomas B. McGuckian,et al.  Development and Validation of a Sensor-Based Algorithm for Detecting the Visual Exploratory Actions , 2018, IEEE Sensors Letters.

[24]  J. Wagman,et al.  Nested prospectivity in perception: Perceived maximum reaching height reflects anticipated changes in reaching ability , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  David N. Lee,et al.  Detecting motor abnormalities in preterm infants , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Wendy D. Zosh,et al.  Optic Flow Drives Human Visuo-Locomotor Adaptation , 2007, Current Biology.

[27]  M. Cole,et al.  Regulation of locomotor pointing across the lifespan: Investigating age-related influences on perceptual-motor coupling , 2018, PloS one.

[28]  J. Vickers Perception, Cognition, and Decision Training: The Quiet Eye in Action , 2007 .

[29]  D. Araújo,et al.  Affordances can invite behavior: Reconsidering the relationship between affordances and agency , 2012 .

[30]  Michael H Cole,et al.  A systematic review of the technology-based assessment of visual perception and exploration behaviour in association football , 2018, Journal of sports sciences.

[31]  Claudia Carello,et al.  Unnerving Intelligence , 2012 .

[32]  F. Zaal,et al.  Shaping decisions in volleyball: an ecological approach to decision-making in volleyball passing. , 2013 .

[33]  David N. Lee,et al.  VISUAL CONTROL OF VELOCITY OF APPROACH BY PIGEONS WHEN LANDING , 1993 .

[34]  K. Adolph,et al.  Psychophysical assessment of toddlers' ability to cope with slopes. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Thomas A. Stoffregen,et al.  Affordances Are Enough: Reply to Chemero et al. (2003) , 2003 .

[36]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[37]  M. Cole,et al.  Associations Between Gait-Related Falls and Gait Adaptations When Stepping Onto a Curb: A Prospective Falls Study. , 2019, Journal of aging and physical activity.

[38]  David N. Lee,et al.  Regulation of gait in long jumping. , 1982 .

[39]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Neville A. Stanton,et al.  Situation awareness: where have we been, where are we now and where are we going? , 2010 .

[41]  Sebastian Madgwick,et al.  Estimation of IMU and MARG orientation using a gradient descent algorithm , 2011, 2011 IEEE International Conference on Rehabilitation Robotics.

[42]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[43]  William H Warren,et al.  How do animals get about by vision? Visually controlled locomotion and orientation after 50 years. , 2009, British journal of psychology.

[44]  D. N. Lee,et al.  Guiding the swing in golf putting , 2000, Nature.

[45]  D. N. Lee,et al.  Neonatal control of nutritive sucking pressure: evidence for an intrinsic τ-guide , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  Jonathan Westley Peirce,et al.  Neuroinformatics Original Research Article Generating Stimuli for Neuroscience Using Psychopy , 2022 .

[47]  Michael H. Cole,et al.  Visual Exploration When Surrounded by Affordances: Frequency of Head Movements Is Predictive of Response Speed , 2018, Ecological Psychology.

[48]  W. Young,et al.  Evidence of a Link Between Fall-Related Anxiety and High-Risk Patterns of Visual Search in Older Adults During Adaptive Locomotion , 2019, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[49]  David N. Lee,et al.  Plummeting gannets: a paradigm of ecological optics , 1981, Nature.

[50]  David N. Lee General Tau Theory: evolution to date. , 2009, Perception.

[51]  E. Freedman Coordination of the eyes and head during visual orienting , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  A. V. D. van der Meer,et al.  TauG-guidance of dynamic balance control during gait initiation across adulthood. , 2012, Gait & posture.

[53]  Kasey C. Soska,et al.  Postural position constrains multimodal object exploration in infants. , 2014, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[54]  Jinwu Qian,et al.  Age- and Parkinson’s disease-related evaluation of gait by General Tau Theory , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  S. T. Rodrigues,et al.  What information is being acquired during the period of Quiet Eye? Comment on Vickers , 2016, Current Issues in Sport Science (CISS).