Perceptual compression of visual space during eye-head gaze shifts.

In primates, inspection of a visual scene is typically interrupted by frequent gaze shifts, occurring at an average rate of three to five times per second. Perceptually, these gaze shifts are accompanied by a compression of visual space toward the saccade target, which may be attributed to an oculomotor signal that transiently influences visual processing. While previous studies of compression have focused exclusively on saccadic eye movements made with the head artificially immobilized, many brain structures involved in saccade generation also encode combined eye-head gaze shifts. Thus, in order to understand the interaction between gaze motor and visual signals, we studied perception during eye-head gaze shifts and found a powerful compression of visual space that was spatially directed toward the intended gaze (and not the eye movement) target location. This perceptual compression was nearly constant in duration across gaze shift amplitudes, suggesting that the signal that triggers compression is largely independent of the size and kinematics of the gaze shift. The spatial pattern of results could be captured by a model that involves interactions, on a logarithmic map of visual space, between two loci of neural activity that encode the gaze shift vector and visual stimulus position relative to the fovea.

[1]  L. Matin,et al.  Visual Perception of Direction for Stimuli Flashed During Voluntary Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.

[2]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Subcortical Modulation of Attention Counters Change Blindness , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Daniel Guitton,et al.  Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head-Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control , 2006, Neuron.

[4]  D. Guitton,et al.  Gaze control in humans: eye-head coordination during orienting movements to targets within and beyond the oculomotor range. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[6]  Lance M. Optican,et al.  Unix-based multiple-process system, for real-time data acquisition and control , 1982 .

[7]  Paul Dassonville,et al.  The use of egocentric and exocentric location cues in saccadic programming , 1995, Vision Research.

[8]  J. Schlag,et al.  Illusory localization of stimuli flashed in the dark before saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[9]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space , 2000, Nature.

[10]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Differential influence of attention on gaze and head movements. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  M. Concetta Morrone,et al.  Apparent Position of Visual Targets during Real and Simulated Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Functional Identification of a Pulvinar Path from Superior Colliculus to Cortical Area MT , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Gellman,et al.  Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  E. Schwartz The development of specific visual connections in the monkey and the goldfish: outline of a geometric theory of receptotopic structure. , 1977, Journal of theoretical biology.

[15]  Markus Lappe,et al.  The Peri-Saccadic Perception of Objects and Space , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[16]  Fred H. Hamker,et al.  The spatial distribution of receptive field changes in a model of peri-saccadic perception: Predictive remapping and shifts towards the saccade target , 2010, Vision Research.

[17]  Daniel Guitton,et al.  Firing Patterns in Superior Colliculus of Head-Unrestrained Monkey during Normal and Perturbed Gaze Saccades Reveal Short-Latency Feedback and a Sluggish Rostral Shift in Activity , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  S. Mateeff Saccadic eye movements and localization of visual stimuli , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  Guido Marco Cicchini,et al.  Spatiotemporal Distortions of Visual Perception at the Time of Saccades , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Arnold Ziesche,et al.  Computational models of spatial updating in peri-saccadic perception , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  Hongying Wang,et al.  Contribution of head movement to gaze command coding in monkey frontal cortex and superior colliculus. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Julio C. Martinez-Trujillo,et al.  Frames of Reference for Eye-Head Gaze Commands in Primate Supplementary Eye Fields , 2004, Neuron.

[23]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Control of orienting gaze shifts by the tectoreticulospinal system in the head-free cat. III. Spatiotemporal characteristics of phasic motor discharges. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  A. Opstal,et al.  Perisaccadic mislocalization of visual targets by head-free gaze shifts: visual or motor? , 2008 .

[25]  F. Ottes,et al.  Visuomotor fields of the superior colliculus: A quantitative model , 1986, Vision Research.

[26]  N. Bischof,et al.  [Investigations and considerations of directional perception during voluntary saccadic eye movements]. , 1968, Psychologische Forschung.

[27]  Carsten Finke,et al.  Perisaccadic Compression Correlates with Saccadic Peak Velocity: Differential Association of Eye Movement Dynamics with Perceptual Mislocalization Patterns , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Jefferson E. Roy,et al.  Dissociating Self-Generated from Passively Applied Head Motion: Neural Mechanisms in the Vestibular Nuclei , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[30]  V. Walsh,et al.  Saccade-Contingent Spatial and Temporal Errors are Absent for Saccadic Head Movements , 2005, Cortex.

[31]  E. Freedman Coordination of the eyes and head during visual orienting , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  M. Lappe,et al.  Contrast dependency of saccadic compression and suppression , 2004, Vision Research.

[33]  James R Müller,et al.  Microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention without moving the eyes. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[35]  A. Sereno,et al.  Disruption of Reflexive Attention and Eye Movements in an Individual with a Collicular Lesion , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[36]  M. Husain,et al.  Vision: Visual space is not what it appears to be , 2001, Current Biology.

[37]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Inactivation of primate superior colliculus impairs covert selection of signals for perceptual judgments , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  Mark D. Semon,et al.  POSTUSE REVIEW: An Introduction to Error Analysis: The Study of Uncertainties in Physical Measurements , 1982 .

[39]  Marcus Kaiser,et al.  Perisaccadic Mislocalization Orthogonal to Saccade Direction , 2004, Neuron.

[40]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[41]  D. Guitton,et al.  On the Role of Subcortical Feedback Mechanisms in the Control of Head-Unrestrained Gaze Saccades , 2003 .

[42]  Simon Carlile,et al.  Compression of auditory space during rapid head turns , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  H. Honda Perceptual localization of visual stimuli flashed during saccades , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  L Stark,et al.  Closely spaced saccades. , 1975, Investigative ophthalmology.

[45]  D L Sparks,et al.  Activity of cells in the deeper layers of the superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey: evidence for a gaze displacement command. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Temporal Uncertainty Separates Flashes from Their Background during Saccades , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  R. Wurtz,et al.  What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex. II. Role of the SC-MD-FEF pathway in corollary discharge. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  The Geometry of Perisaccadic Visual Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  H. Honda Saccade-contingent displacement of the apparent position of visual stimuli flashed on a dimly illuminated structured background , 1993, Vision Research.

[50]  D. Guitton,et al.  Control of eye—head coordination during orienting gaze shifts , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.