Teammates first: Favoring ingroup robots over outgroup humans
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Muzafer Sherif,et al. The Robbers Cave experiment : intergroup conflict and cooperation , 1988 .
[2] Selma Sabanovic,et al. Rabble of Robots Effects: Number and Type of Robots Modulates Attitudes, Emotions, and Stereotypes , 2015, 2015 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).
[3] Friederike Eyssel,et al. Minimal Group - Maximal Effect? Evaluation and Anthropomorphization of the Humanoid Robot NAO , 2011, ICSR.
[4] N. Haslam,et al. Attributing and denying humanness to others , 2008 .
[5] A. Greenwald,et al. With malice toward none and charity for some: ingroup favoritism enables discrimination. , 2014, The American psychologist.
[6] H. Tajfel,et al. Social categorization and intergroup behaviour , 1971 .
[7] Sau-lai Lee,et al. Hitting a robot vs. hitting a human: Is it the same? , 2011, 2011 6th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).
[8] Selma Sabanovic,et al. Negative Attitudes toward minimalistic Robots with intragroup communication styles , 2014, The 23rd IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.
[9] Lowell Gaertner,et al. When Rejection by One Fosters Aggression Against Many: Multiple-Victim Aggression as a Consequence of Social Rejection and Perceived Groupness. , 2008, Journal of experimental social psychology.
[10] Friederike Eyssel,et al. ‘If you sound like me, you must be more human’: On the interplay of robot and user features on human-robot acceptance and anthropomorphism , 2012, 2012 7th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).
[11] R. Baumeister,et al. If you can't join them, beat them: effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[12] Selma Sabanovic,et al. Colleague or Tool? Interactivity Increases Positive Perceptions of and Willingness to Interact with a Robotic Co-worker , 2016, ICSR.
[13] N. Haslam,et al. Animals and Androids , 2007, Psychological science.
[14] Christoph Bartneck,et al. “Daisy, daisy, give me your answer do!” switching off a robot , 2007, 2007 2nd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).
[15] Marjorie Rhodes,et al. Social Categories as Markers of Intrinsic Interpersonal Obligations , 2013, Psychological science.
[16] Friederike Eyssel,et al. When a Robot’s Group Membership Matters , 2013, International Journal of Social Robotics.
[17] Kurt Gray,et al. Mind Perception Is the Essence of Morality , 2012, Psychological inquiry.
[18] Brian P. Meier,et al. A comparison of human aggression committed by groups and individuals: An interindividual–intergroup discontinuity☆ , 2004 .
[19] Melvin Manis,et al. Shifting standards and stereotype-based judgments. , 1994 .
[20] N. Haslam,et al. Dehumanization and infrahumanization. , 2014, Annual review of psychology.
[21] D. Wegner,et al. Causes and consequences of mind perception , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[22] Emanuele Castano,et al. Morality shifting in the context of intergroup violence , 2012 .
[23] P. W. Singer,et al. Robots at War , 2011 .
[24] D. Wegner,et al. Dimensions of Mind Perception , 2007, Science.
[25] Takayuki Kanda,et al. The new ontological category hypothesis in human-robot interaction , 2011, 2011 6th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).
[26] F. Eyssel,et al. (S)he's Got the Look: Gender Stereotyping of Robots1 , 2012 .
[27] Selma Sabanovic,et al. Getting in Touch: How imagined, actual, and physical contact affect evaluations of robots , 2016, 2016 25th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN).