Acute effects of breaking up prolonged sitting on fatigue and cognition: a pilot study

Objectives To compare the acute effects of uninterrupted sitting with sitting interrupted by brief bouts of light-intensity walking on self-reported fatigue, cognition, neuroendocrine biomarkers and cardiometabolic risk markers in overweight/obese adults. Design Randomised two-condition crossover trial. Setting Laboratory study conducted in Melbourne, Australia. Participants 19 overweight/obese adults (45–75 years). Interventions After an initial 2 h period seated, participants consumed a meal-replacement beverage and completed (on 2 days separated by a 6-day washout period) each condition over the next 5 h: uninterrupted sitting (sedentary condition) or sitting with 3 min bouts of light-intensity walking every 30 min (active condition). Primary outcome measures Self-reported fatigue, executive function and episodic memory at 0 h, 4 h and 7 h. Secondary outcome measures Neuroendocrine biomarkers and cardiometabolic risk markers (blood collections at 0 h, 4 h and 7 h, blood pressure and heart rate measured hourly and interstitial glucose measured using a continuous glucose monitoring system). Results During the active condition, fatigue levels were lower at 4 h (−13.32 (95% CI −23.48 to −3.16)) and at 7 h (−10.73 (95% CI −20.89 to −0.58)) compared to the sedentary condition. Heart rate was higher at 4 h (4.47 (95% CI 8.37 to 0.58)) and at 7 h (4.32 (95% CI 8.21 to 0.42)) during the active condition compared to the sedentary condition. There were no significant differences between conditions by time for other variables. In the sedentary condition, changes in fatigue scores over time correlated with a decrease in heart rate and plasma dihydroxyphenylalanine (DOPA) and an increase in plasma dihydroxyphenylglycol (DHPG). Conclusions Interrupting prolonged sitting with light-intensity walking breaks may be an effective fatigue countermeasure acutely. Fatigue levels corresponded with the heart rate and neuroendocrine biomarker changes in uninterrupted sitting in this pilot study. Further research is needed to identify potential implications, particularly for the occupational health context. Trial registration number ACTRN12613000137796; Results.

[1]  N. Owen,et al.  Breaking up prolonged sitting reduces resting blood pressure in overweight/obese adults. , 2014, Nutrition, metabolism, and cardiovascular diseases : NMCD.

[2]  Sanjay Kalra,et al.  OBJECTIVELY MEASURED LIGHT‐INTENSITY PHYSICAL ACTIVITY IS INDEPENDENTLY ASSOCIATED WITH 2‐H PLASMA GLUCOSE , 2008 .

[3]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[4]  A. Friedman,et al.  Systematic review of sedentary behavior and cognitive development in early childhood. , 2015, Preventive medicine.

[5]  L. Mccargar,et al.  Test-retest reliability of a continuous glucose monitoring system in individuals with type 2 diabetes. , 2014, Diabetes technology & therapeutics.

[6]  Ronald J Sigal,et al.  Point accuracy of interstitial continuous glucose monitoring during exercise in type 1 diabetes. , 2013, Diabetes technology & therapeutics.

[7]  L. Riby,et al.  Lifestyle, glucose regulation and the cognitive effects of glucose load in middle-aged adults. , 2008, The British journal of nutrition.

[8]  J. Izzo,et al.  Plasma dihydroxyphenylglycol (DHPG) in the in vivo assessment of human neuronal norepinephrine metabolism. , 1985, Life sciences.

[9]  N. Owen,et al.  Impact on hemostatic parameters of interrupting sitting with intermittent activity. , 2013, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[10]  E. McAuley,et al.  Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  M. Meeus,et al.  Malfunctioning of the autonomic nervous system in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome: a systematic literature review , 2014, European journal of clinical investigation.

[12]  David W Dunstan,et al.  Breaking up workplace sitting time with intermittent standing bouts improves fatigue and musculoskeletal discomfort in overweight/obese office workers , 2014, Occupational and Environmental Medicine.

[13]  J. Zoladz,et al.  The effect of physical activity on the brain derived neurotrophic factor: from animal to human studies. , 2010, Journal of physiology and pharmacology : an official journal of the Polish Physiological Society.

[14]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[15]  W. Tamborlane,et al.  A tale of two compartments: interstitial versus blood glucose monitoring. , 2009, Diabetes technology & therapeutics.

[16]  Leon Straker,et al.  The contribution of office work to sedentary behaviour associated risk , 2013, BMC Public Health.

[17]  G. Borg Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. , 1982, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[18]  Laura D. Ellingson,et al.  Active and sedentary behaviors influence feelings of energy and fatigue in women. , 2014, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[19]  D. Atlas,et al.  The Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors , 1994, The Receptors.

[20]  N. Murray,et al.  Acute Physical Activity on Cognitive Function: A Heart Rate Variability Examination , 2012, Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback.

[21]  Gabriel A. Koepp,et al.  Using Sit-Stand Workstations to Decrease Sedentary Time in Office Workers: A Randomized Crossover Trial , 2014, International journal of environmental research and public health.

[22]  Lars Nyberg,et al.  Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[23]  Doerte U. Junghaenel,et al.  Demographic correlates of fatigue in the US general population: results from the patient-reported outcomes measurement information system (PROMIS) initiative. , 2011, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[24]  Cedric L. Williams,et al.  Epinephrine administration increases neural impulses propagated along the vagus nerve: Role of peripheral β-adrenergic receptors , 2006, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[25]  K. Radeborg,et al.  Effects of differences in postprandial glycaemia on cognitive functions in healthy middle-aged subjects , 2009, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[26]  Maarten A. S. Boksem,et al.  Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[27]  P S Freedson,et al.  Calibration of the Computer Science and Applications, Inc. accelerometer. , 1998, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[28]  David B. Hogan,et al.  Cerebrovascular Reserve: The Link Between Fitness and Cognitive Function? , 2012, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[29]  R. Beck,et al.  Use of continuous glucose monitoring as an outcome measure in clinical trials. , 2012, Diabetes technology & therapeutics.

[30]  Daniel P. Bailey,et al.  Breaking up prolonged sitting with light-intensity walking improves postprandial glycemia, but breaking up sitting with standing does not. , 2015, Journal of science and medicine in sport.

[31]  C. Matthews,et al.  Identifying sedentary time using automated estimates of accelerometer wear time , 2011, British Journal of Sports Medicine.

[32]  T. Weaver,et al.  Sleepiness, physical activity, and functional outcomes in veterans with type 2 diabetes. , 2009, Applied nursing research : ANR.

[33]  I. Jonsdottir,et al.  Influence of voluntary exercise on hypothalamic norepinephrine. , 1998, Journal of applied physiology.

[34]  J. Shaw,et al.  Breaking Up Prolonged Sitting Reduces Postprandial Glucose and Insulin Responses , 2012, Diabetes Care.

[35]  Schofield Wn,et al.  Predicting basal metabolic rate, new standards and review of previous work , 1985 .

[36]  Emmanuel Stamatakis,et al.  Prospective study of sedentary behavior, risk of depression, and cognitive impairment. , 2014, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[37]  Kouzi Yamaguti,et al.  Autonomic nervous alterations associated with daily level of fatigue , 2011, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[38]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[39]  W. Kirchner Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  G. Wylie,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of fatigue. , 2009, Physical medicine and rehabilitation clinics of North America.

[41]  Kathryn A. Lee,et al.  Validity and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue , 1991, Psychiatry Research.

[42]  Stephen M Stahl,et al.  The many faces of fatigue in major depressive disorder. , 2005, The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology.

[43]  R. Mazze,et al.  Evaluating the accuracy, reliability, and clinical applicability of continuous glucose monitoring (CGM): Is CGM ready for real time? , 2009, Diabetes technology & therapeutics.

[44]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Preserved hippocampus activation in normal aging as revealed by fMRI , 2011, Hippocampus.

[45]  T. Dang-Vu,et al.  The Role of Sleep and Physical Activity on the Risk for Cardiovascular Disease , 2014, Current Cardiovascular Risk Reports.

[46]  Natasha M. Maurits,et al.  Mental Fatigue Affects Visual Selective Attention , 2012, PloS one.

[47]  J. Etnier,et al.  The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis , 2012, Brain Research.

[48]  Gordon Broderick,et al.  Biomarkers for chronic fatigue , 2012, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

[49]  B. Popkin,et al.  Time use and physical activity: a shift away from movement across the globe , 2012, Obesity reviews : an official journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity.

[50]  A. B. Scholey,et al.  Glucose administration, heart rate and cognitive performance: effects of increasing mental effort , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[51]  T. McMorris,et al.  Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: A meta-analytical investigation , 2012, Brain and Cognition.