Empirical Analysis of Three Dimensions of Spoken Discourse: Segmentation, Coherence, and Linguistic Devices

A discourse consists not simply of a linear sequence of utterances,1 but of meaningful semantic or pragmatic relations among utterances. Each utterance of a discourse either bears a relation to a preceding utterance or constitutes the onset of a new unit of meaning or action that subsequent utterances may add to. The need to model the relation between such units and linguistic features of utterances is almost universally acknowledged in the literature on discourse. For example, previous work argues for an interdependence between particular cue words and phrases such as anyway, and their location relative to an utterance or text, (e.g., Hirschberg and Litman 1993, Grosz and Sidner 1986, Reichman 1985, Cohen 1984); the distribution and duration of pauses relative to multi-utterance units (e.g., Grosz and Hirschberg 1992, Hirschberg and Grosz 1992, Chafe 1980, Butterworth 1980); and the interdependence between the form of discourse anaphoric noun phrases and the relation of the current utterance to a hierarchical model of utterance actions, or to a model of focus of attention (e.g., Grosz 1977, Grosz and Sidner 1986, Reichman 1985, Sidner 1979, Passonneau 1985). However, there are a variety of distinct proposals regarding how to model the interdependence among the three dimensions of: 1. sequences of semantically and pragmatically related utterances, 2. the units or relations they reflect, and 3. lexico-grammatical or prosodic features. We refer to these dimensions respectively as segmentation, coherence, and linguistic devices.

[1]  Robin Cohen,et al.  A Computational Theory of the Function of Clue Words in Argument Understanding , 1984, ACL.

[2]  Rebecca J. Passonneau,et al.  Intention-Based Segmentation: Human Reliability and Correlation with Linguistic Cues , 1993, ACL.

[3]  Johanna D. Moore,et al.  A Problem for RST: The Need for Multi-Level Discourse Analysis , 1992, CL.

[4]  Rebecca J. Passonneau,et al.  Empirical Evidence for Intention-Based Discourse Segmentation , 1993, Workshop On Intentionality And Structure In Discource Relations.

[5]  John Andrew Rotondo,et al.  Clustering analysis of subjective partitions of text , 1984 .

[6]  Rebecca J. Passonneau,et al.  Getting and keeping the center of attention , 1993 .

[7]  Lauri Karttunen,et al.  Discourse Referents , 1969, COLING.

[8]  Rebecca J. Schiffman,et al.  Discourse constraints on ‘it'and ‘that': a study of language use in career-counseling interviews , 1985 .

[9]  Candace L. Sidner,et al.  Attention, Intentions, and the Structure of Discourse , 1986, CL.

[10]  J. Austin How to do things with words , 1962 .

[11]  J. Pierrehumbert The phonology and phonetics of English intonation , 1987 .

[12]  Klaus Krippendorff,et al.  Content Analysis: An Introduction to Its Methodology , 1980 .

[13]  William C. Mann,et al.  Rhetorical Structure Theory: Toward a functional theory of text organization , 1988 .

[14]  Rachel Reichman,et al.  Getting computers to talk like you and me , 1985 .

[15]  Julia Hirschberg,et al.  Intonational Features of Local and Global Discourse Structure , 1992, HLT.

[16]  Madeleine Bates,et al.  Challenges in natural language processing: Conclusion , 1993 .

[17]  Candace L. Sidner,et al.  Towards a computational theory of definite anaphora comprehension in English discourse , 1979 .

[18]  James D. McCawley,et al.  Notes from the linguistic underground , 1978 .

[19]  S. Thompson,et al.  Discourse description : diverse linguistic analyses of a fund-raising text , 1992 .

[20]  W. G. Cochran The comparison of percentages in matched samples. , 1950, Biometrika.

[21]  Johanna D. Moore,et al.  Planning Text for Advisory Dialogues: Capturing Intentional and Rhetorical Information , 1993, CL.

[22]  E. Schegloff,et al.  Opening up Closings , 1973 .

[23]  Julia Hirschberg,et al.  Some intonational characteristics of discourse structure , 1992, ICSLP.

[24]  Jerry R. Hobbs Coherence and Coreference , 1979, Cogn. Sci..

[25]  R. J. Lickley,et al.  Proceedings of the International Conference on Spoken Language Processing. , 1992 .

[26]  Jeroen Groenendijk,et al.  Formal methods in the study of language. part 2 , 1981 .

[27]  Bonnie L. Webber,et al.  Structure and Ostension in the Interpretation of Discourse Deixis , 1991, ArXiv.

[28]  W. Chafe The Pear Stories: Cognitive, Cultural and Linguistic Aspects of Narrative Production , 1980 .

[29]  Marti A. Hearst Multi-Paragraph Segmentation Expository Text , 1994, ACL.

[30]  Barbara J. Grosz,et al.  The representation and use of focus in dialogue understanding. , 1977 .

[31]  David Yarowsky,et al.  Estimating Upper and Lower Bounds on the Performance of Word-Sense Disambiguation Programs , 1992, ACL.

[32]  Barbara A. Fox Discourse Structure and Anaphora: References , 1987 .

[33]  L. Polanyi A formal model of the structure of discourse , 1988 .

[34]  Julia Hirschberg,et al.  Empirical Studies on the Disambiguation of Cue Phrases , 1993, Comput. Linguistics.

[35]  Graeme Hirst,et al.  Lexical Cohesion Computed by Thesaural relations as an indicator of the structure of text , 1991, CL.

[36]  William C. Mann,et al.  Rhetorical structure theory and text analysis , 1989 .

[37]  Greg Carlson,et al.  A unified analysis of the English bare plural , 1977 .

[38]  Christoph Schwarze,et al.  Meaning, Use, and Interpretation of Language , 1983 .