Linguistic Intergroup Bias: Stereotype Perpetuation Through Language

Publisher Summary Language is considered as the major means by which stereotypes are communicated through interpersonal discourse, by which they are transmitted from generation to generation, and by which the press and other mass media create social representations of social groups. Language also constitutes the principal means by which sociologists and social-psychologists tend to measure stereotypes. Language abstraction plays a subtle but important role in stereotype transmission and maintenance. The aim of this chapter is to describe language abstraction and its development over the past years. The chapter discusses its implications and proposes extensions of the model to related areas. The linguistic intergroup bias (LIB) model describes a systematic bias in language use, which can contribute to the perpetuation of stereotypes. The chapter describes the research paradigm most frequently employed in LIB studies, reviews empirical findings testing the main predictions, addresses questions of external validity, compares hypotheses about the underlying mechanisms of the LIB, speculate about extensions of the model beyond intergroup relations, and finally discusses a number of open problems that are of interest.

[1]  V. Dijk,et al.  Prejudice In Discourse , 1984 .

[2]  A. Maass,et al.  On the (dis-)confirmability of stereotypic attributes , 1998 .

[3]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Social categorization and memory for in-group and out-group behavior. , 1980 .

[4]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound , 1994 .

[5]  G. Semin,et al.  Language use in the context of congruent and incongruent in-group behaviours , 1994 .

[6]  Patrick T. Vargas,et al.  On The Role Of Encoding Processes In Stereotype Maintenance , 1995 .

[7]  A. Maass,et al.  Intentional control over prejudice: when the choice of the measure matters , 1999 .

[8]  H. Giles,et al.  Ethnolinguistic identity theory: a social psychological approach to language maintenance , 1987 .

[9]  D. Funder,et al.  Differences between traits: properties associated with interjudge agreement. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  M. Rothbart,et al.  On the confirmability and disconfirmability of trait concepts. , 1986 .

[11]  Patrick T. Vargas,et al.  The Linguistic Intergroup Bias As an Implicit Indicator of Prejudice , 1997 .

[12]  A. Maass,et al.  Linguistic intergroup bias: differential expectancies or in-group protection? , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  A. Maass,et al.  Linguistic intergroup bias and implicit attributions , 1993 .

[14]  S. Ng Androcentric coding of man and his in memory by language users , 1990 .

[15]  G. Āllport The Nature of Prejudice , 1954 .

[16]  K. Fiedler,et al.  The Answer is in the Question: The Effect of Verb Causality on Locus of Explanation , 1995 .

[17]  Gün R. Semin,et al.  The linguistic category model, its bases, applications and range , 1991 .

[18]  R. Fazio,et al.  Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Janet B. Ruscher,et al.  Prejudice and Stereotyping in Everyday Communication , 1998 .

[20]  A. Maass,et al.  The role of language in the persistence of stereotypes. , 1992 .

[21]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Motivated language use in intergroup contexts: need-for-closure effects on the linguistic intergroup bias. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  A. Maass,et al.  Implicit Versus Explicit Strategies of Out-Group Discrimination , 1996 .

[23]  Janice Moulton,et al.  Sex bias in language use: "Neutral" pronouns that aren't. , 1978 .

[24]  D. Wigboldus,et al.  Children's communication of the Linguistic Intergroup Bias and its impact upon cognitive inferences , 1999 .

[25]  G. Semin,et al.  The question–answer paradigm: You might regret not noticing how a question is worded. , 1997 .

[26]  N. Vanbeselaere The Different Effects of Simple and Crossed Categorizations: A Result of the Category Differentiation Process or of Differential Category Salience? , 1991 .

[27]  A. Maass,et al.  Linguistic intergroup bias: Evidence for in-group-protective motivation. , 1996 .

[28]  G. Semin,et al.  Language use in intergroup contexts: the linguistic intergroup bias. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  B Guerin Gender bias in the abstractness of verbs and adjectives. , 1994, The Journal of social psychology.

[30]  S. Worchel,et al.  Psychology of intergroup relations , 1986 .

[31]  K. Fiedler,et al.  The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. , 1988 .

[32]  Donald M. Taylor,et al.  Review of Theories of Intergroup Relations: International Social Psychological Perspectives , 1987 .

[33]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Perceiving persons and groups. , 1996, Psychological review.

[34]  W. Hippel,et al.  The role of the linguistic intergroup bias in expectancy maintenance , 1996 .

[35]  J. Bargh,et al.  Context-dependent automatic processing in depression: accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  N. Vanbeselaere The impact of differentially valued overlapping categorizations upon the differentiation between positively, negatively, and neutrally evaluated social groups , 1996 .

[37]  Gün R. Semin,et al.  Language, interaction and social cognition , 1992 .

[38]  Daniel Bar-Tal,et al.  Stereotyping and prejudice: Changing conceptions. , 1989 .

[39]  S. Fulero,et al.  Recall for confirming events: Memory processes and the maintenance of social stereotypes ☆ , 1979 .

[40]  K. Fiedler,et al.  The Battle of Words Between Gender Groups A Language‐Based Approach to Intergroup Processes , 1993 .

[41]  T. V. Dijk,et al.  Communicating Racism: Ethnic Prejudice in Thought and Talk , 1987 .