Bilateral cutaneous stimulation of the somatosensory system in hemidecorticate rats.

An enduring somatosensory consequence of extensive neocortex injury in people is "simultaneous extinction," which is an interhemispheric perceptual interaction that is operationally distinguishable from neglect. A cutaneous stimulus presented on the contralateral side of the body is readily detected when presented singly but is actively masked during bilateral stimulation. In hemidecorticate rats, small adhesive stimuli were attached to the radial surface of each forelimb simultaneously, and the latencies to contact and to remove each stimulus were recorded. Neglect of the contralateral stimulus lasted 2-3 days. Thereafter the ipsilateral stimulus was removed first, followed immediately by the contralateral stimulus. This ipsilateral sensorimotor bias lasted several months. Further analysis with unremovable tactile stimuli provided evidence for true extinction. For a limited period (during the first 2 postoperative weeks), the contralateral stimulus did not appear to be detected in the presence of the ipsilateral stimulus. Rather than switching back and forth between the two stimuli, the animals ignored the stimulus on the contralateral limb and persisted in their attempts to remove the ipsilateral stimulus. A key feature of the "extinction" was its complete reversibility. Simply by adjusting the sensory fields occupied by the contralateral (C) and ipsilateral (I) stimuli (specifically, by increasing the C/I ratio), the sensorimotor bias was totally shifted to contralateral. During recovery, the size of the C/I ratio necessary to reverse sensorimotor asymmetry gradually decreased. Asymmetrical sensorimotor behavior and amphetamine-induced circling were examined in three additional groups of hemidecorticate rats, which were given their initial behavioral tests at 2, 12, or 52 postoperative weeks. This experiment confirmed the above findings and controlled for practice. Over the course of a year, apparently complete recovery occurred; however, there were residual effects. At each period, previously recovered symptoms were reinstated simply by turning on the room lights and opening the home cage slightly. These data underscore the importance of detailed behavioral analysis and the value of the rat as a model in studies of recovery of sensorimotor function.

[1]  E. Eidelberg,et al.  “Extinction” to bilateral simultaneous stimulation in the monkey , 1968, Neurology.

[2]  R. Dykes Parallel Processing of Cutaneous Information in the Somatosensory System of the Cat , 1982, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[3]  T. Ljungberg,et al.  Sensory inattention produced by 6-hydroxydopamine-induced degeneration of ascending dopamine neurons in the brain , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[4]  J. Hekmatpanah Organization of tactile dermatomes, C1 through L4, in cat. , 1961, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  K. Heilman,et al.  A unilateral memory defect 1 , 1974, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[6]  A. Benton,et al.  An experimental study of 'obscuration' , 1972, Neurology.

[7]  P. Teitelbaum,et al.  Nigrostriatal bundle damage and the lateral hypothalamic syndrome. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[8]  J. Coyle,et al.  Lateralization of catecholaminergic and behavioral response to cerebral infarction in the rat. , 1979, Life sciences.

[9]  A. Schwartz,et al.  The asymmetric lateralization of tactile extinction in patients with unilateral cerebral dysfunction. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  K. Welch,et al.  Experimental production of unilateral neglect in monkeys. , 1958, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  R. A. Chambers,et al.  The parietal lobe and behavior. , 1958, Research publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease.

[12]  I. Hanin,et al.  Animal models in psychiatry and neurology , 1977 .

[13]  O. L. Zangwill,et al.  VISUAL-CONSTRUCTIVE DISABILITIES ASSOCIATED WITH LESIONS OF THE LEFT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE , 1960 .

[14]  W. R. Brain VISUAL DISORIENTATION WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO LESIONS OF THE RIGHT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE , 1941 .

[15]  L. Bianchi THE FUNCTIONS OF THE FRONTAL LOBES , 1895 .

[16]  N. Lobaugh,et al.  Tactile extinction: Distinguishing between sensorimotor and motor asymmetries in rats with unilateral nigrostriatal damage , 1982, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[17]  G. Békésy,et al.  Experiments in Hearing , 1963 .

[18]  V. Edgerton,et al.  Rapid ankle extension during paw shakes: selective recruitment of fast ankle extensors. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Robert C. Collins,et al.  Recovery from unilateral neglect , 1983, Experimental Neurology.

[20]  S. Kiefer,et al.  The gustatory neocortex of the rat , 1982 .

[21]  K. Heilman,et al.  Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect , 1979, Annals of neurology.

[22]  E. Eidelberg,et al.  Experimental analysis of the extinction phenomenon in monkeys. , 1971, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  T. Schallert,et al.  Posture-independent sensorimotor analysis of inter-hemispheric receptor asymmetries in neostriatum , 1983, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[24]  Joachim F. R. König,et al.  The rat brain: A stereotaxic atlas of the forebrain and lower parts of the brain stem , 1986 .

[25]  T. Schallert Adipsia produced by lateral hypothalamic lesions: facilitation of recovery by preoperative restriction of water intake. , 1982, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[26]  D. Denny-Brown,et al.  The significance of perceptual rivalry resulting from parietal lesion. , 1952, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  C. Yeo,et al.  The effects of unilateral frontal eye field lesions in the monkey: Visual-motor guidance and avoidance behaviour , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.

[28]  B BenderMorris,et al.  DISORDERS IN PERCEPTION , 1954 .

[29]  P. Nathan,et al.  On simultaneous bilateral stimulation of the body in a lesion of the parietal lobe. , 1946, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  S. Finger,et al.  Brain Damage and Recovery: Research and Clinical Perspectives , 1983 .

[31]  C Luzzatti,et al.  Unilateral neglect, representational schema and consciousness. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  I. Whishaw,et al.  An analysis of feeding and sensorimotor abilities of rats after decortication. , 1981, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[33]  H. Teuber,et al.  Tactile Extinction in Parietal Lobe Neoplasm , 1954 .

[34]  D. S. Feldman,et al.  Double Simultaneous Stimulation Phenomena in Spinal Cord Disease , 1952, Neurology.

[35]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[36]  P. Teitelbaum,et al.  Atropine stereotypy as a behavioral trap: a movement subsystem and electroencephalographic analysis. , 1980, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[37]  U. Ungerstedt,et al.  Quantitative recording of rotational behavior in rats after 6-hydroxy-dopamine lesions of the nigrostriatal dopamine system. , 1970, Brain research.

[38]  A. Benton Jacques Loeb and the method of double stimulation. , 1956, Journal of the history of medicine and allied sciences.

[39]  J. Lynch The functional organization of posterior parietal association cortex , 1980, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[40]  Dr. Juhani Hyvärinen The Parietal Cortex of Monkey and Man , 1982, Studies of Brain Function.