Confidence-enhanced Performance * Olivier Compte Ceras

There is ample evidence that emotions affect performance. Positive emotions can improve performance, while negative ones may diminish it. For example, the fears induced by the possibility of failure or of negative evaluations have physiological consequences (shaking, loss of concentration) that may impair performance in sports, on stage or at school. There is also ample evidence that individuals have distorted recollection of past events, and distorted attributions of the causes of successes of failures. Recollection of good events or successes is typically easier than recollection of bad ones or failures. Successes tend to be attributed to intrinsic aptitudes or own effort, while failures are attributed to bad luck. In addition, these attributions are often reversed when judging the performance of others. The objective of this paper is to incorporate the first phenomenon above into an otherwise standard decision theoretic model, and show that in a world where performance depends on emotions, biases in information processing enhance welfare.

[1]  M L Russell,et al.  Differential effects of induced mood on the recall of positive, negative and neutral words. , 1983, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[2]  R. Dolan,et al.  The interaction between mood and cognitive function studied with PET , 1997, Psychological Medicine.

[3]  Hanming Fang Confidence Management and the Free-Riding Problem ∗ , 2001 .

[4]  M. Ross,et al.  Egocentric Biases in Availability and Attribution , 1979 .

[5]  J. Tirole,et al.  Self-Confidence and Personal Motivation , 2002 .

[6]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Well-being : the foundations of hedonic psychology , 1999 .

[7]  M. Rabin Published by: American , 2022 .

[8]  Claude M. Steele,et al.  How stereotypes influence the standardized test performance of talented African American students , 1998 .

[9]  E. Steen Skill or Luck? Biases of Rational Agents , 2002 .

[10]  L. Ross,et al.  Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. , 1981 .

[11]  O. Svenson ARE WE ALL LESS RISKY AND MORE SKILLFUL THAN OUR FELLOW DRIVERS , 1981 .

[12]  J. Geanakoplos,et al.  Psychological games and sequential rationality , 1989 .

[13]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[14]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Knowing with Certainty: The Appropriateness of Extreme Confidence. , 1977 .

[15]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Calibration of probabilities: the state of the art to 1980 , 1982 .

[16]  L. Samuelson Information-Based Relative Consumption Effects , 2004 .

[17]  Lynn A. Baker,et al.  When every relationship is above average , 1993 .

[18]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[19]  C. Steele,et al.  Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  H. Ellis,et al.  Emotion, motivation, and text comprehension: the detection of contradictions in passages. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[22]  Michael Waldman,et al.  Systematic Errors and the Theory of Natural Selection , 1994 .

[23]  W. Morris,et al.  The mood system. , 1999 .

[24]  A. Oving,et al.  Induced Mood and Selective Attention , 1997, Perceptual and motor skills.

[25]  Joel L. Schrag,et al.  First Impressions Matter: A Model of Confirmatory Bias , 1999 .

[26]  M T Reinitz,et al.  A demonstration and comparison of two types of inference-based memory errors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  Catherine Good,et al.  When White Men Can't Do Math: Necessary and Sufficient Factors in Stereotype Threat , 1999 .

[28]  F. McKenna,et al.  A speech rate measure of laboratory induced affect: the role of demand characteristics revisited. , 1994, The British journal of clinical psychology.