The development of geographic categories and biases.

Children and university students (N=58) estimated the locations of major cities in North America. At age 9, a distinct home region was apparent, but no differentiation between northern US and Canadian cities. At 11, four developments were observed: Children divided North America into regions that were not based solely on national boundaries but were the same as university students' regions; psychological border zones between regions exaggerated distances between them; children used new location information to update their estimates for all cities in a seeded region and in adjacent and nonadjacent regions; children preserved the ordinal structure of their initial location estimates for cities in their home region but relied on regional prototype locations to adjust estimates in less familiar regions. The updating methods reflect fundamentally different mechanisms. Theoretical and educational implications are discussed.

[1]  David Stea,et al.  Mapping as a Cultural Universal , 1996 .

[2]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  Structural descriptions and the limitations of visual images* , 1974, Memory & cognition.

[3]  W. A. Bousfield,et al.  An Analysis of Sequences of Restricted Associative Responses , 1944 .

[4]  D. H. Wheeler,et al.  The early growth of logic in the child : classification and seriation , 1965 .

[5]  J. Plumert,et al.  The development of memory for location: what role do spatial prototypes play? , 2001, Child development.

[6]  J. Dixon,et al.  Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 2001 .

[7]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  A new look at anchoring effects: basic anchoring and its antecedents. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  Albert C. Stevens,et al.  Distortions in judged spatial relations , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  G. Jahoda THE DEVELOPMENT OF CHILDREN'S IDEAS ABOUT COUNTRY AND NATIONALITY: Part II: National Symbols and Themes , 1963 .

[10]  R. Siegler,et al.  Long-term benefits of seeding the knowledge base , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  H. Wellman,et al.  Young Children's Representation of Spatial Information Acquired From Maps , 1989 .

[12]  G. Jahoda CHILDREN'S CONCEPTS OF NATIONALITY: A CRITICAL STUDY OF PIAGET'S STAGES. , 1964, Child development.

[13]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Spatial Scaling in Young Children , 1999 .

[14]  Lynn S. Liben,et al.  Developing an understanding of graphic representations in children and adults: The case of GEO-graphics , 1992 .

[15]  A Friedman,et al.  Reasoning about geography. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  D. Uttal Preschoolers’ and adults’ scale translation and reconstruction of spatial information acquired from maps , 1994 .

[17]  R. Downs,et al.  Image and Environment: Cognitive Mapping and Spatial Behavior , 2017 .

[18]  T. McNamara,et al.  Subjective hierarchies in spatial memory. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  F. Strack,et al.  Explaining the Enigmatic Anchoring Effect: Mechanisms of Selective Accessibility , 1997 .

[20]  G. Winer,et al.  Class-Inclusion Reasoning in Children: A Review of the Empirical Literature. , 1980 .

[21]  J. Portugali The construction of cognitive maps , 1996 .

[22]  L. Hedges,et al.  Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. , 1991, Psychological review.

[23]  A. Friedman,et al.  Updating geographical knowledge: principles of coherence and inertia. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Advanced Spatial Skills and Advance Planning: Components of 6-Year-Olds' Navigational Map Use , 2001 .

[25]  T. Saarinen,et al.  World Patterns of Geographic Literacy Based on Sketch Map Quality , 1995 .

[26]  S. Golbeck,et al.  Preschoolers' knowledge of their classroom environment: evidence from small-scale and life-size spatial tasks. , 1982, Child development.

[27]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Measures of Anchoring in Estimation Tasks , 1995 .

[28]  Joseph Glicksohn,et al.  Rotation, orientation, and cognitive mapping , 1994 .

[29]  Lynn S. Liben,et al.  Understanding Person-Space-Map Relations: Cartographic and Developmental Perspectives. , 1993 .

[30]  R. Siegler,et al.  Metrics and mappings: a framework for understanding real-world quantitative estimation. , 1993, Psychological review.

[31]  L. E. Bourne,et al.  Memory seeding: representations underlying quantitative estimations. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  Alinda Friedman,et al.  Spatial location judgments: A cross-national comparison of estimation bias in subjective North American geography , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  B. Tversky Distortions in memory for maps , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Barrier effects in the cognitive maps of children and adults. , 1982, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[35]  Lynn S. Liben,et al.  Fostering geographic literacy from early childhood: the contributions of interdisciplinary research , 1994 .

[36]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[37]  David H. Uttal,et al.  A Cross‐Cultural Study of Young Children's Mapping Abilities , 1998 .

[38]  J. Pinheiro Determinants of cognitive maps of the world as expressed in sketch maps , 1998 .

[39]  R. Siegler,et al.  Seeds aren’t anchors , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[40]  Alinda Friedman,et al.  A basis for bias in geographical judgments , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  R. Downs,et al.  On Education and Geographers: The Role of Cognitive Developmental Theory in Geographic Education , 1988 .

[42]  N. Brown Real-world estimation: Estimation modes and seeding effects , 2002 .

[43]  R. Maki Categorization and distance effects with spatial linear orders. , 1981 .