Cognitive control adjustments and conflict adaptation in major depressive disorder.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Peter E Clayson | Michael J Larson | Peter E. Clayson | M. Larson | A. Clawson | Ann Clawson | P. Clayson
[1] D. Sheehan,et al. DSM-IH-R Psychotic Disorders: procedural validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). Concordance and causes for discordance with the CIDI , 1998, European Psychiatry.
[2] Robert F. Simons,et al. Anxiety and error-related brain activity , 2003, Biological Psychology.
[3] Nachshon Meiran,et al. Cognitive rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder: Examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and post-conflict adaptation , 2011, Psychiatry Research.
[4] Cameron S Carter,et al. Development of action monitoring through adolescence into adulthood: ERP and source localization. , 2007, Developmental science.
[5] T. Egner,et al. Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[6] H. Schimmel. The (�) Reference: Accuracy of Estimated Mean Components in Average Response Studies , 1967, Science.
[7] P Videbech,et al. PET measurements of brain glucose metabolism and blood flow in major depressive disorder: a critical review , 2000, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.
[8] S. Moritz,et al. Executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia , 2002 .
[9] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[10] A. Holmes,et al. Effects of task-relevant incentives on the electrophysiological correlates of error processing in major depressive disorder , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[11] A. Zuardi,et al. Validity and applicability of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview administered by family medicine residents in primary health care in Brazil. , 2008, General hospital psychiatry.
[12] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[13] Christopher J. May,et al. Is “Blank” a suitable neutral prime for event-related potential experiments? , 2006, Brain and Language.
[14] Lorna C. Quandt,et al. Error detection and posterror behavior in depressed undergraduates. , 2008, Emotion.
[15] Peter E. Clayson,et al. Performance Monitoring and Cognitive Control in Individuals with Mild Traumatic Brain Injury , 2012, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[16] A. Vincent,et al. Distractibility and processing resource deficit in major depression. Evidence for two deficient attentional processing models. , 1997, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.
[17] Avram J Holmes,et al. Task feedback effects on conflict monitoring and executive control: relationship to subclinical measures of depression. , 2007, Emotion.
[18] Scott A. Baldwin,et al. Performance monitoring following conflict: Internal adjustments in cognitive control? , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[19] R. Davidson,et al. Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.
[20] W. Hulstijn,et al. Drug-induced stimulation and suppression of action monitoring in healthy volunteers , 2004, Psychopharmacology.
[21] T. Egner. Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[22] Anna Weinberg,et al. Increased error-related brain activity in generalized anxiety disorder , 2010, Biological Psychology.
[23] H J Keselman,et al. A generally robust approach to hypothesis testing in independent and correlated groups designs. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[24] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Resting anterior cingulate activity and abnormal responses to errors in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms: A 128‐channel EEG study , 2006, Human brain mapping.
[25] K. Meador,et al. Clinical assessment of Axis I psychiatric morbidity in chronic epilepsy: a multicenter investigation. , 2005, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.
[26] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring , 2006, Psychological science.
[27] W. Hulstijn,et al. The impact of perfectionism and anxiety traits on action monitoring in major depressive disorder , 2010, Journal of Neural Transmission.
[28] M. Falkenstein,et al. ERP indices for response inhibition are related to anxiety-related personality traits , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[29] R. West,et al. Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[30] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[31] Anna Weinberg,et al. Event‐related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: Temporospatial principal components analysis and source localization of the feedback negativity , 2011, Human brain mapping.
[32] D. Sheehan,et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. , 1998, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.
[33] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[34] C. Carter,et al. The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.
[35] Michael J. Larson,et al. Sex differences in electrophysiological indices of conflict monitoring , 2011, Biological Psychology.
[36] Avram J. Holmes,et al. Response conflict and frontocingulate dysfunction in unmedicated participants with major depression , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[37] H. Mayberg,et al. An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[38] Markus Kiefer,et al. Error processing in major depressive disorder: evidence from event-related potentials. , 2006, Journal of psychiatric research.
[39] R. C. Fraley,et al. Is depression best viewed as a continuum or discrete category? A taxometric analysis of childhood and adolescent depression in a population-based sample. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[40] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.
[41] G. Goodwin,et al. Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible implications for functional neuropathology , 2001, British Journal of Psychiatry.
[42] Peter E. Clayson,et al. How does noise affect amplitude and latency measurement of event-related potentials (ERPs)? A methodological critique and simulation study. , 2013, Psychophysiology.
[43] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Parametric Manipulation of the Conflict Signal and Control-state Adaptation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] W. Hulstijn,et al. Action monitoring and depressive symptom reduction in major depressive disorder. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[45] C. Carter,et al. The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[46] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.
[47] Lauren M. Bylsma,et al. The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[48] William M. Perlstein,et al. Neural time course of conflict adaptation effects on the Stroop task , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[49] R. Raedt,et al. Impairments in cognitive control persist during remission from depression and are related to the number of past episodes: An event related potentials study , 2009, Biological Psychology.
[50] Robert F. Simons,et al. Electrophysiological indicators of emotion processing biases in depressed undergraduates , 2009, Biological Psychology.
[51] C. Beevers,et al. Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[52] Joseph Dien,et al. The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[53] Peter E. Clayson,et al. Effects of repetition priming on electrophysiological and behavioral indices of conflict adaptation and cognitive control. , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[54] E. Foa,et al. Increased error-related brain activity in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder before and after treatment. , 2008, The American journal of psychiatry.
[55] W. Gehring,et al. Action-Monitoring Dysfunction in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 2000, Psychological science.
[56] R. Cattell. The Scree Test For The Number Of Factors. , 1966, Multivariate behavioral research.
[57] T. Robbins,et al. Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis specific impairment , 1997, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[58] Ronald C Kessler,et al. The categorical versus dimensional assessment controversy in the sociology of mental illness. , 2002, Journal of health and social behavior.
[59] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.
[60] Paul E. Croarkin,et al. Psychomotor retardation in depression: Biological underpinnings, measurement, and treatment , 2011, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
[61] W. Nager,et al. Oxazepam alters action monitoring , 2001, Psychopharmacology.
[62] Peter E. Clayson,et al. The effects of induced state negative affect on performance monitoring processes. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[63] Joseph Dien,et al. Evaluation of PCA and ICA of simulated ERPs: Promax vs. infomax rotations , 2007, Human brain mapping.
[64] Avram J Holmes,et al. Spatiotemporal dynamics of error processing dysfunctions in major depressive disorder. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.
[65] W. Hulstijn,et al. Effects of antipsychotic and antidepressant drugs on action monitoring in healthy volunteers , 2006, Brain Research.
[66] Affective regulation of cognitive-control adjustments in remitted depressive patients after acute tryptophan depletion , 2012, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[67] Jan R. Wessel,et al. Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[68] T. Endrass,et al. Overactive performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ERP evidence from correct and erroneous reactions , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[69] M. Fleck,et al. Subsyndromal depression: an impact on quality of life? , 2007 .
[70] Zhengzhi Feng,et al. Deficient interference inhibition for negative stimuli in depression: An event-related potential study , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[71] P. Amorim. Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): validacao de entrevista breve para diagnostico de transtornos mentais , 2000 .
[72] Peter E. Clayson,et al. Cognitive control in mild traumatic brain injury: conflict monitoring and conflict adaptation. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[73] Karl J. Friston,et al. The anatomy of melancholia – focal abnormalities of cerebral blood flow in major depression , 1992, Psychological Medicine.
[74] M. Junghöfer,et al. The polar average reference effect: a bias in estimating the head surface integral in EEG recording , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[75] L. Judd,et al. Subsyndromal symptomatic depression: a new mood disorder? , 1994, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.
[76] Doreen M. Olvet,et al. Depression symptom severity and error-related brain activity , 2010, Psychiatry Research.
[77] Jonathan R. Folstein,et al. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[78] Wouter Hulstijn,et al. Action monitoring in major depressive disorder with psychomotor retardation , 2008, Cortex.
[79] Tobias Egner,et al. Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Mediates Individual Differences in Conflict-driven Cognitive Control , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[80] J. Horn. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis , 1965, Psychometrika.
[81] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[82] Joseph Dien,et al. Separating the visual sentence N400 effect from the P400 sequential expectancy effect: Cognitive and neuroanatomical implications , 2010, Brain Research.
[83] Tobias Egner,et al. The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task , 2005, NeuroImage.
[84] Peter E. Clayson,et al. Cognitive performance and electrophysiological indices of cognitive control: a validation study of conflict adaptation. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[85] Peter E. Clayson,et al. Conflict adaptation and sequential trial effects: Support for the conflict monitoring theory , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[86] Peter E. Clayson,et al. The relationship between cognitive performance and electrophysiological indices of performance monitoring , 2011, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.