Nonrandom Dispersal in Free-Ranging Vervet Monkeys: Social and Genetic Consequences

Male vervet monkeys in Amboseli National Park, Kenya, disperse nonrandomly from their natal groups at sexual maturity, and migrate to specific neighboring groups with their brothers or peers. Nonrandom dispersal in the company of allies appears to benefit young males by minimizing the risk of predation and reducing the probability of attack by resident males and females. Nonrandom dispersal also decreases the likelihood of mating with close female kin. Persistent nonrandom transfer, however, increases the risk of inbreeding depression. We emphasize the importance of considering social, as well as genetic, factors when evaluating the costs and benefits of any pattern of dispersal. In certain cases, there may be social advantages to a particular dispersal pattern even though it appears initially to carry some genetic costs. In other cases, a dispersal pattern that at first appears to increase the risk of inbreeding may in fact permit individuals to avoid mating with close kin. Finally, the benefits of a given pattern of dispersal may change markedly during an individual's life. While males seem to benefit by transferring nonrandomly with their peers when young, it may be more advantageous for older males to disperse alone. Age-related changes in the social benefits of nonrandom transfer appear to have important genetic consequences for the population as a whole.

[1]  C. Mcmillan,et al.  Interlineage genetic differentiation among rhesus macaques on Cayo Santiago. , 1981, American journal of physical anthropology.

[2]  P. Slater,et al.  Interest of mice in conspecific male odours is influenced by degree of kinship , 1978, Nature.

[3]  B. Bertram,et al.  Male lions in large coalitions gain reproductive advantages , 1979, Nature.

[4]  Fernando Nottebohm,et al.  THE SONG OF THE CHINGOLO, ZOiVOTRICHlA CAPENSIS, IN ARGENTINA: DESCRIPTION AND EVALUATION OF A SYSTEM OF DIALECTS , 1969 .

[5]  G. Frame,et al.  Female African wild dogs emigrate , 1976, Nature.

[6]  C. Marsh Female transference and mate choice among Tana River red colobus , 1979, Nature.

[7]  P. Greenwood Mating systems, philopatry and dispersal in birds and mammals , 1980, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  Ching Chun Li The Effects of Inbreeding on Japanese Children , 1966 .

[9]  G. Dahlberg,et al.  Genetics of human populations. , 1948, Advances in genetics.

[10]  D. G. Smith Inbreeding in three captive groups of rhesus monkeys. , 1982, American journal of physical anthropology.

[11]  E. Seemanová A study of children of incestuous matings. , 1971, Human heredity.

[12]  D. Lindburg Rhesus Monkeys: Mating Season Mobility of Adult Males , 1969, Science.

[13]  J. Walters Inferring kinship from behaviour: Maternity determinations in yellow baboons , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[14]  M. Kimura,et al.  The Stepping Stone Model of Population Structure and the Decrease of Genetic Correlation with Distance. , 1964, Genetics.

[15]  J. Hoogland,et al.  Prairie Dogs Avoid Extreme Inbreeding , 1982, Science.

[16]  J. Buettner‐Janusch,et al.  Carbonic anhydrase heterozygosity and FST distributions in Kenyan baboon troops. , 1978, American journal of physical anthropology.

[17]  C. Packer Inter-troop transfer and inbreeding avoidance in Papio anubis , 1979, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  B. Bengtsson Avoiding inbreeding: at what cost? , 1978, Journal of theoretical biology.

[19]  S. P. Henzi,et al.  Observations on the Inter-troop movement of adult vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). , 1980, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[20]  J. Cheverud,et al.  Social group fission and the origin of intergroup genetic differentiation among the rhesus monkeys of Cayo Santiago. , 1978, American journal of physical anthropology.

[21]  I. Devore,et al.  Population dynamcs of rhesus monkeys on Cayo Santiago , 1965 .

[22]  T. Enomoto The sexual behavior of Japanese monkeys , 1974 .

[23]  ERNEST CRAWLEY,et al.  Kinship and Marriage , 1904, Nature.

[24]  L L Cavalli-Sforza,et al.  A migration matrix model for the study of random genetic drift. , 1968, Genetics.

[25]  J A Kurland,et al.  Kin selection in the Japanese monkey. , 1977, Contributions to primatology.

[26]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Recognition of Individuals Within and Between Groups of Free-Ranging Vervet Monkeys1 , 1982 .

[27]  A. Harcourt Strategies of emigration and transfer by primates, with particular reference to gorillas. , 2010, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.

[28]  T. Feizi Blood Group Antigens , 1975, Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[29]  R. Wrangham Drinking competition in vervet monkeys , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  Y. Sugiyama Life history of male Japanese monkeys , 1976 .

[31]  L. Partridge,et al.  Olfactory preferences of inbred mice (Mus musculus) for their own strain and for siblings: effects of strain, sex and cross-fostering. , 1981 .

[32]  R. Wrangham,et al.  FEEDING BEHAVIOUR OF VERVET MONKEYS ON ACACIA TORTILIS AND ACACIA XANTHOPHLOEA: WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO REPRODUCTIVE STRATEGIES AND TANNIN PRODUCTION , 1981 .

[33]  C. Packer,et al.  Cooperation and competition within coalitions of male lions: kin selection or game theory? , 1982, Nature.

[34]  S. Vessey,et al.  Nepotism among rhesus monkey brothers , 1981, Nature.

[35]  R. May When to be incestuous , 1979, Nature.

[36]  A. King,et al.  Song structure and song development as potential contributors to reproductive isolation in cowbirds (Molothrus ater). , 1980 .