Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. W. van der Molen,et al. An additive factors analysis of the effect(s) of location cues associated with auditory stimuli on stages of information processing. , 1989, Acta psychologica.
[2] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] J. C. Johnston,et al. Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference , 1992 .
[4] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Differential effects of central and peripheral cues on the reorienting of spatial attention. , 1991 .
[5] R W Proctor,et al. Referential coding and attention-shifting accounts of the Simon effect , 1994, Psychological research.
[6] J. Richard Simon,et al. The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on choice reaction time: Duration of the phenomenon and its relation to stages of processing , 1976 .
[7] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. , 1995 .
[8] B Hommel,et al. The role of attention for the Simon effect , 1993, Psychological research.
[9] R. Wallace,et al. S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[10] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[11] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[12] C. Umilta. Orienting of attention. , 1988 .
[13] K. Heilman,et al. Attentional factors in the occurrence of stimulus-response compatibility effects , 1988, Neuropsychologia.
[14] Jordan Grafman,et al. Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .
[15] T. Stoffer. Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility , 1991, Psychological research.
[16] S Kornblum,et al. The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli , 1994, Psychological research.
[17] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[19] A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al. The role of position in object selection in vision , 1993 .
[20] Carlo Umiltà,et al. An integrated model of the Simon effect. , 1992 .
[21] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[22] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes , 1994, Psychological research.
[23] J. R. Simon,et al. Response selection in choice reaction time: Test of a buffer model , 1980, Memory & cognition.
[24] D. Holender,et al. Analytic approaches to human cognition , 1992 .
[25] T. Stoffer,et al. The functional role of attention for spatial coding in the Simon effect , 1994, Psychological research.
[26] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[27] C. Eriksen,et al. Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.
[28] R. Chua,et al. Attention shifts and the Simon effect: a failure to replicate Stoffer (1991) , 1996 .
[29] Jason B. Mattingley,et al. Hand-hemispace spatial compatibility, precueing, and stimulus-onset asynchrony , 1994, Psychological research.
[30] Raja Parasuraman,et al. Varieties of attention , 1984 .
[31] J R Simon,et al. Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.
[32] M. Zorzi,et al. A computational model of the Simon effect , 1995, Psychological research.
[33] G. d'Ydewalle,et al. Effects of multiple reference points in spatial stimulus-response compatibility. , 1992, Acta psychologica.
[34] Y. Tsal,et al. Location dominance in attending to color and shape. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .
[36] C. Umilta,et al. Splitting visual space with attention. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] L. Dell’Osso,et al. Saccadic system plasticity in humans , 1978, Annals of neurology.
[38] T. Stoffer. Attentional zooming and the global-dominance phenomenon: Effects of level-specific cueing and abrupt visual onset , 1994, Psychological research.
[39] J. R. Simon. The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .
[40] G. Logan. Linguistic and Conceptual Control of Visual Spatial Attention , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.
[41] L. Zimba,et al. Attention precuing and Simon effects: A test of the attention-coding account of the Simon effect , 1995 .
[42] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Egocentric and relative spatial codes in S-R compatibility , 1987 .
[43] W. Becker,et al. An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.
[44] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[45] B. Hommel. Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.
[46] B. Hommel. The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap , 1993 .
[47] W. Schneider. VAM: A neuro-cognitive model for visual attention control of segmentation, object recognition, and space-based motor action , 1995 .
[48] T Hasbroucq,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: toward a conceptual clarification. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.