A Computational Theory of Learning Causal Relationships

I present a cognitive model of the human ability to acquire causal relationships. I report on experimental evidence demonstrating that human learners acquire accurate causal relationships more rapidly when training examples are consistent with a general theory of causality. This article describes a learning process that uses a general theory of causality as background knowledge. The learning process, which I call theory-driven learning (TDL), hypothesizes causal relationships consistent both with observed data and the general theory of causality. TDL accounts for data on both the rate at which human learners acquire causal relationships, and the types of causal relationships they acquire. Experiments with TDL demonstrate the advantage of TDL for acquiring causal relationships over similarity-based approaches to learning: Fewer examples are required to learn an accurate relationship.

[1]  T. Shultz,et al.  The Use of Covariation as a Principle of Causal Analysis. , 1975 .

[2]  Tom M. Mitchell,et al.  Generalization as Search , 2002 .

[3]  S. Vere Induction of concepts in the predicate calculus , 1975, IJCAI 1975.

[4]  J. Piaget The Child's Conception of Physical Causality , 1927 .

[5]  T. Shultz Rules of Causal Attribution. , 1982 .

[6]  Thomas R. Shultz,et al.  Selection of Causal Rules. , 1986 .

[7]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning internal representations by error propagation , 1986 .

[8]  C. Hartshorne,et al.  Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce , 1935, Nature.

[9]  Thomas G. Dietterich Learning at the knowledge level , 2004, Machine Learning.

[10]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  SCRIPTS, PLANS, GOALS, AND UNDERSTANDING , 1988 .

[12]  G. Bower,et al.  Attention in Learning: Theory and Research , 1968 .

[13]  Michael Lebowitz,et al.  Integrated Learning: Controlling Explanation , 1986, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  Michael G. Dyer,et al.  Using Prior Learning to Facilitate the Learning of New Causal Theories , 1987, IJCAI.

[15]  Steven Salzberg Heuristics for Inductive Learning , 1985, IJCAI.

[16]  D. Ausubel,et al.  The effect of incidental and experimentally induced experience in the learning of relevant and irrelevant causal relationships by children. , 1954, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[17]  M. Levine The size of the hypothesis set during discrimination learning. , 1967, Psychological review.

[18]  A. Leslie,et al.  Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? , 1987, Cognition.

[19]  Janet L. Kolodner,et al.  Retrieval and organizational strategies in conceptual memory: a computer model , 1980 .

[20]  Putnam,et al.  The Collected Papers. , 1988 .

[21]  N. Goodman Fact, Fiction, and Forecast , 1955 .

[22]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[23]  A. Michotte The perception of causality , 1963 .

[24]  Michael Lebowitz,et al.  Not the Path to Perdition: The Utility of Similarity-Based Learning , 1986, AAAI.

[25]  岡本 栄一,et al.  Tom Trabasso & Gardon H. Bower-Attention in Learning : Theory and Research., John Wiley and Sons, 1968, 234ページ , 1972 .

[26]  William M. Smith,et al.  A Study of Thinking , 1956 .

[27]  Michael Lebowitz,et al.  Generalization and memory in an integrated understanding system , 1980 .