Frontoparietal activation with preparation for antisaccades.

Several current models hold that frontoparietal areas exert cognitive control by biasing task-relevant processing in other brain areas. Previous event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have compared prosaccades and antisaccades, which require subjects to look toward or away from a flashed peripheral stimulus, respectively. These studies found greater activation for antisaccades in frontal and parietal regions at the ends of long (>or=6 s) preparatory periods preceding peripheral stimulus presentation. Event-related fMRI studies using short preparatory periods (<or=4 s) have not found such activation differences except in the frontal eye field. Here, we identified activation differences associated with short (1-s) preparatory periods by interleaving half trials among regular whole trials in a rapid fMRI design. On whole trials, a colored fixation dot instructed human subjects to make either a prosaccade toward or an antisaccade away from a peripheral visual stimulus. Half trials included only the instruction and not peripheral stimulus presentation or saccade generation. Nonetheless, half trials evoked stronger activation on antisaccades than on prosaccades in the frontal eye field (FEF), supplementary eye field (SEF), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and precuneus. Greater antisaccade response-related activation was found in FEF, SEF, IPS, and precuneus but not in DLPFC or ACC. These results demonstrate greater preparatory activation for antisaccades versus prosaccades in frontoparietal areas and suggest that prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex are more involved in presetting the saccade network for the antisaccade task than generating the actual antisaccade response.

[1]  S. Everling,et al.  Cortical potentials preceding pro- and antisaccades in man. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[2]  Edward V. Evarts,et al.  Electromyography in CNS Disorders , 1985, Neurology.

[3]  D. Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Monkey Superior Colliculus Related to the Initiation of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  A transmit‐only/receive‐only (TORO) RF system for high‐field MRI/MRS applications , 2000, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[5]  Kristen A. Ford,et al.  Inhibition and generation of saccades: Rapid event-related fMRI of prosaccades, antisaccades, and nogo trials , 2006, NeuroImage.

[6]  K. Johnston,et al.  Top-Down Control-Signal Dynamics in Anterior Cingulate and Prefrontal Cortex Neurons following Task Switching , 2007, Neuron.

[7]  Yan Wang,et al.  Cingulate cortical cells projecting to monkey frontal eye field and primary motor cortex , 2004, Neuroreport.

[8]  Y Agid,et al.  Cortical control of reflexive visually-guided saccades. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  R. Douglas,et al.  Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Canceling planned action: an FMRI study of countermanding saccades. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  D. Munoz,et al.  Influence of stimulus eccentricity and direction on characteristics of pro- and antisaccades in non-human primates. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Christoph Klein,et al.  Impaired modulation of the saccadic contingent negative variation preceding antisaccades in schizophrenia , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  E. Seidemann,et al.  Dynamics of Depolarization and Hyperpolarization in the Frontal Cortex and Saccade Goal , 2002, Science.

[16]  J. Schall,et al.  Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  S. Everling,et al.  The antisaccade: a review of basic research and clinical studies , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A general statistical analysis for fMRI data , 2000, NeuroImage.

[19]  B. Gaymard,et al.  The frontal eye field is involved in spatial short-term memory but not in reflexive saccade inhibition , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  W. H. Howell,et al.  A TEXTBOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY , 1934 .

[22]  M. Sereno,et al.  Mapping of Contralateral Space in Retinotopic Coordinates by a Parietal Cortical Area in Humans , 2001, Science.

[23]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Effects of cortical lesions on saccadic: eye movements in humans. , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  Christoph J. Ploner,et al.  Effects of Cortical Lesions on Saccadic , 2002 .

[25]  Clayton E Curtis,et al.  Selection and maintenance of saccade goals in the human frontal eye fields. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[27]  C. Curtis,et al.  Success and Failure Suppressing Reflexive Behavior , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Raymond M. Klein,et al.  The Magnitude of the Fixation Offset Effect with Endogenously and Exogenously Controlled Saccades , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  K. Johnston,et al.  Monkey Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Sends Task-Selective Signals Directly to the Superior Colliculus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  D P Munoz,et al.  Role of Primate Superior Colliculus in Preparation and Execution of Anti-Saccades and Pro-Saccades , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Jun Zhang,et al.  Flexible filaments in a flowing soap film as a model for one-dimensional flags in a two-dimensional wind , 2000, Nature.

[33]  E C Wong,et al.  Processing strategies for time‐course data sets in functional mri of the human brain , 1993, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[34]  T A Carpenter,et al.  Colored noise and computational inference in neurophysiological (fMRI) time series analysis: Resampling methods in time and wavelet domains , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[35]  Sabine Kastner,et al.  Topographic maps in human frontal cortex revealed in memory-guided saccade and spatial working-memory tasks. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  B. Fischer,et al.  Characteristics of “anti” saccades in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Decisional role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behaviour. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  John T. Serences,et al.  A comparison of methods for characterizing the event-related BOLD timeseries in rapid fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[39]  G. Glover,et al.  Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  The Prefrontal Substrate of Reflexive Saccade Inhibition in Humans , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[41]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Activity of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey during an antisaccade task , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI I. The Method , 2001, NeuroImage.

[43]  G. Leichnetz,et al.  The prefrontal corticotectal projection in the monkey; An anterograde and retrograde horseradish peroxidase study , 1981, Neuroscience.

[44]  B. Gaymard,et al.  Effects of anterior cingulate cortex lesions on ocular saccades in humans , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in humans investigated with event-related FMRI. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  Persistent LIP activity in memory antisaccades: working memory for a sensorimotor transformation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Heike Weber,et al.  Effects of stimulus conditions on the performance of antisaccades in man , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Rule-dependent Activity for Prosaccades and Antisaccades in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[51]  D. Heeger,et al.  Linear Systems Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Human V1 , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  L Martyn Klassen,et al.  Robust automated shimming technique using arbitrary mapping acquisition parameters (RASTAMAP) , 2004, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[53]  Matthew R. G. Brown,et al.  Neural processes associated with antisaccade task performance investigated with event-related FMRI. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[55]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI II. Analysis , 2001, NeuroImage.

[56]  T. Vilis,et al.  Remapping the remembered target location for anti-saccades in human posterior parietal cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  Madeleine Schlag-Rey,et al.  Primate antisaccade. II. Supplementary eye field neuronal activity predicts correct performance. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Chantal François,et al.  A direct prefrontotectal tract against distractibility in the human brain , 2003, Annals of neurology.

[59]  W. B. Saunders A Textbook of Physiology , 1932, Nature.

[60]  J. Schall Visuomotor Areas of the Frontal Lobe , 1997 .

[61]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Neural Activity in Monkey Prefrontal Cortex Is Modulated by Task Context and Behavioral Instruction during Delayed-match-to-sample and Conditional ProsaccadeAntisaccade Tasks , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  D. Heeger,et al.  Sustained Activity in Topographic Areas of Human Posterior Parietal Cortex during Memory-Guided Saccades , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  John Duncan,et al.  Frontoparietal Activity with Minimal Decision and Control , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[66]  I. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .

[67]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades , 2000, Nature.

[68]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Human fMRI evidence for the neural correlates of preparatory set , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[69]  S. Everling,et al.  Event-related potentials associated with correct and incorrect responses in a cued antisaccade task , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task , 1980, Vision Research.