Communicating tailored risk information of cancer treatment side effects: Only words or also numbers?

Background The increased availability of patient reported outcome data makes it feasible to provide patients tailored risk information of cancer treatment side effects. However, it is unclear how such information influences patients’ risk interpretations compared to generic population-based risks, and which message format should be used to communicate such individualized statistics. Methods A web-based experiment was conducted in which participants ( n  = 141) read a hypothetical treatment decision-making scenario about four side effect risks of adjuvant chemotherapy for advanced colon cancer. Participants were cancer patients or survivors who were recruited from an online Dutch cancer patient panel. All participants received two tailored risks (of which the reference class was based on their age, gender and tumor stage) and two generic risks conveying the likelihood of experiencing the side effects. The risks were presented either in words-only (‘common’ and ‘very common’), or in a combination of words and corresponding numerical estimates (‘common, 10 out of 100’ and ‘very common, 40 out of 100’). Participants’ estimation of the probability, accuracy of their estimation, and perceived likelihood of occurrence were primary outcomes. Perceived personal relevance and perceived uncertainty were secondary outcomes. Results Tailored risks were estimated as higher and less accurate than generic risks, but only when they were presented in words; Such differences were not found in the verbal and numerical combined condition. Although tailoring risks did not impact participants’ perceived likelihood of occurrence, tailored risks were perceived as more personally relevant than generic risks in both message formats. Finally, tailored risks were perceived as less uncertain than generic risks, but only in the verbal-only condition. Conclusions Considering current interest in the use of personalized decision aids for improving shared decision-making in oncology, it is important that clinicians consider how tailored risks of treatment side effects should be communicated to patients. We recommend both clinicians who communicate probability information during consultations, and decision aid developers, that verbal descriptors of tailored risks should be supported by numerical estimates of risks levels, to avoid overestimation of risks.

[1]  P. Ubel,et al.  Measuring Numeracy without a Math Test: Development of the Subjective Numeracy Scale , 2007, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[2]  Lane Harrison,et al.  PROACT: Iterative Design of a Patient-Centered Visualization for Effective Prostate Cancer Health Risk Communication , 2017, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[3]  Caitlin Gutheil,et al.  The value of personalised risk information: a qualitative study of the perceptions of patients with prostate cancer , 2013, BMJ Open.

[4]  A. Stiggelbout,et al.  Probabilities of benefit and harms of preoperative radiotherapy for rectal cancer: What do radiation oncologists tell and what do patients understand? , 2015, Patient education and counseling.

[5]  S. Noar,et al.  A Meta-Analysis of Web-Delivered Tailored Health Behavior Change Interventions , 2013, Journal of health communication.

[6]  D. Spiegelhalter Risk and Uncertainty Communication , 2017 .

[7]  Gergely Csibra,et al.  Why do we remember? The communicative function of episodic memory , 2017, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[8]  P. Gardner,et al.  Perceived risk of tamoxifen side effects: a study of the use of absolute frequencies or frequency bands, with or without verbal descriptors. , 2010, Patient education and counseling.

[9]  M. Galesic,et al.  Who profits from visual aids: overcoming challenges in people's understanding of risks [corrected]. , 2010, Social science & medicine.

[10]  Vivianne H M Visschers,et al.  Probability Information in Risk Communication: A Review of the Research Literature , 2009, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[11]  Erika A. Waters,et al.  Risk Estimates From an Online Risk Calculator Are More Believable and Recalled Better When Expressed as Integers , 2011, Journal of medical Internet research.

[12]  W. Klein,et al.  The effect of peer comparison information in the context of expert recommendations on risk perceptions and subsequent behavior , 2009 .

[13]  L. V. van de Poll-Franse,et al.  “Am I normal?” The Wishes of Patients With Lymphoma to Compare Their Patient-Reported Outcomes With Those of Their Peers , 2017, Journal of medical Internet research.

[14]  S. Sansgiry,et al.  Communicating risk of medication side-effects: role of communication format on risk perception , 2018, Pharmacy practice.

[15]  Alexander Etz,et al.  Making replication mainstream , 2017, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  Peter H. Schwartz Disclosure and rationality: Comparative risk information and decision-making about prevention , 2009, Theoretical medicine and bioethics.

[17]  Robert Fletcher,et al.  Tailored Computer-Based Cancer Risk Communication: Correcting Colorectal Cancer Risk Perception , 2004, Journal of health communication.

[18]  R. Thomson,et al.  Shared Decision Making: A Model for Clinical Practice , 2012, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[19]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[20]  Borsika A. Rabin,et al.  Development, Feasibility, and Small-Scale Implementation of a Web-Based Prognostic Tool—Surveillance, Epidemiology, and End Results Cancer Survival Calculator , 2017, JMIR cancer.

[21]  Roland Büchter,et al.  Words or numbers? Communicating risk of adverse effects in written consumer health information: a systematic review and meta-analysis , 2014, BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making.

[22]  Hilde van der Togt,et al.  Publisher's Note , 2003, J. Netw. Comput. Appl..

[23]  Paul K. J. Han,et al.  Presenting quantitative information about decision outcomes: a risk communication primer for patient decision aid developers , 2013, BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making.

[24]  Combined verbal and numerical expressions increase perceived risk of medicine side‐effects: a randomized controlled trial of EMA recommendations , 2016, Health expectations : an international journal of public participation in health care and health policy.

[25]  Angela Fagerlin,et al.  "If I'm better than average, then I'm ok?": Comparative information influences beliefs about risk and benefits. , 2007, Patient education and counseling.

[26]  M. Bouvy,et al.  Preferences of Patients and Pharmacists with Regard to the Management of Drug–Drug Interactions: A Choice-Based Conjoint Analysis , 2017, Drug Safety.

[27]  P. Ubel,et al.  Can avoidance of complications lead to biased healthcare decisions? , 2006, Judgment and Decision Making.

[28]  P. Ubel,et al.  Validation of the Subjective Numeracy Scale: Effects of Low Numeracy on Comprehension of Risk Communications and Utility Elicitations , 2007, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[29]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Designing Visual Aids That Promote Risk Literacy: A Systematic Review of Health Research and Evidence-Based Design Heuristics , 2017, Hum. Factors.

[30]  R. Schilsky Personalized medicine in oncology: the future is now , 2010, Nature Reviews Drug Discovery.

[31]  Lidewij Henneman,et al.  Presenting health risk information in different formats: the effect on participants' cognitive and emotional evaluation and decisions. , 2008, Patient education and counseling.

[32]  H. G. van der Poel,et al.  The accuracy of patients’ perceptions of the risks associated with localised prostate cancer treatments , 2018, BJU International.

[33]  Ingrid Flight,et al.  The information needs of adult cancer survivors across the cancer continuum: A scoping review. , 2017, Patient education and counseling.

[34]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[35]  Liana Fraenkel,et al.  Individualized medical decision making: necessary, achievable, but not yet attainable. , 2010, Archives of internal medicine.

[36]  D. Arenberg,et al.  Evaluation of a Personalized, Web-Based Decision Aid for Lung Cancer Screening. , 2015, American journal of preventive medicine.

[37]  Ruben D. Vromans,et al.  Changes in internet use and wishes of cancer survivors: A comparison between 2005 and 2017 , 2019, Cancer.

[38]  Sarah M. Greene,et al.  Women’s decisions regarding tamoxifen for breast cancer prevention: responses to a tailored decision aid , 2010, Breast Cancer Research and Treatment.

[39]  I. Lipkus Numeric, Verbal, and Visual Formats of Conveying Health Risks: Suggested Best Practices and Future Recommendations , 2007, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[40]  G Pravettoni,et al.  Patient decision aids for prevention and treatment of cancer diseases: are they really personalised tools? , 2016, European journal of cancer care.

[41]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTEREST Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics , 2022 .

[42]  Jeroen Vermunt,et al.  A Personalized Data-to-Text Support Tool for Cancer Patients , 2019, INLG.

[43]  A. Freeman How to communicate evidence to patients , 2019, Drug and Therapeutics Bulletin.

[44]  P. Knapp,et al.  Comparison of two methods of presenting risk information to patients about the side effects of medicines , 2004, Quality and Safety in Health Care.

[45]  C. C. Chandler,et al.  Risk Perception: Using Conditional Versus General Base Rates for Risk Communication1 , 2005 .

[46]  Sandra van Dulmen,et al.  Tailored information about cancer risk and screening: a systematic review. , 2009, Patient education and counseling.

[47]  Andrew N Freedman,et al.  Communication of Uncertainty Regarding Individualized Cancer Risk Estimates , 2011, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[48]  Ruben D. Vromans,et al.  Communicative aspects of decision aids for localized prostate cancer treatment - A systematic review. , 2019, Urologic oncology.

[49]  Ruben D. Vromans,et al.  Assessing the quality and communicative aspects of patient decision aids for early-stage breast cancer treatment: a systematic review , 2019, Breast Cancer Research and Treatment.

[50]  Ying Mai Kung,et al.  A model of tailoring effects: A randomized controlled trial examining the mechanisms of tailoring in a web-based STD screening intervention. , 2016, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[51]  P. Ubel,et al.  Helping patients decide: ten steps to better risk communication. , 2011, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[52]  A. Stiggelbout,et al.  Do clinicians convey what they intend? Lay interpretation of verbal risk labels used in decision encounters. , 2020, Patient education and counseling.

[53]  Martin Härter,et al.  A three-talk model for shared decision making: multistage consultation process , 2017, British Medical Journal.

[54]  J. Bjorner,et al.  Applying PRO Reference Values to Communicate Clinically Relevant Information at the Point-of-care. , 2019, Medical care.

[55]  R. Padman,et al.  Effectiveness of Personalized and Interactive Health Risk Calculators , 2012, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.