Unifying Speed-Accuracy Trade-Off and Cost-Benefit Trade-Off in Human Reaching Movements

Two basic trade-offs interact while our brain decides how to move our body. First, with the cost-benefit trade-off, the brain trades between the importance of moving faster toward a target that is more rewarding and the increased muscular cost resulting from a faster movement. Second, with the speed-accuracy trade-off, the brain trades between how accurate the movement needs to be and the time it takes to achieve such accuracy. So far, these two trade-offs have been well studied in isolation, despite their obvious interdependence. To overcome this limitation, we propose a new model that is able to simultaneously account for both trade-offs. The model assumes that the central nervous system maximizes the expected utility resulting from the potential reward and the cost over the repetition of many movements, taking into account the probability to miss the target. The resulting model is able to account for both the speed-accuracy and the cost-benefit trade-offs. To validate the proposed hypothesis, we confront the properties of the computational model to data from an experimental study where subjects have to reach for targets by performing arm movements in a horizontal plane. The results qualitatively show that the proposed model successfully accounts for both cost-benefit and speed-accuracy trade-offs.

[1]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Statistical decision theory and the selection of rapid, goal-directed movements. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[2]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Stochastic Optimal Control and Estimation Methods Adapted to the Noise Characteristics of the Sensorimotor System , 2005, Neural Computation.

[3]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Landy,et al.  The expected utility of movement , 2009 .

[6]  Mathias Pessiglione,et al.  Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Individual differences in response speed and accuracy are associated to specific brain activities of two interacting systems , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[8]  R Plamondon,et al.  Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  Denis Mottet,et al.  Behind Fitts' law: kinematic patterns in goal-directed movements , 2004, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[10]  Katsumi Aoki,et al.  Recent development of flow visualization , 2004, J. Vis..

[11]  M. Desmurget,et al.  Computational motor control: feedback and accuracy , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  T. S. Constantinidis,et al.  Speed-accuracy trade-off in the performance of pointing movements in different directions in two-dimensional space , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Yves Guiard,et al.  Fitts' law in two-dimensional task space , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  The speed-accuracy trade-off in manual prehension: effects of movement amplitude, object size and object width on kinematic characteristics , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Masazumi Katayama,et al.  Virtual trajectory and stiffness ellipse during multijoint arm movement predicted by neural inverse models , 1993, Biological Cybernetics.

[16]  Mark Dean,et al.  Trading off speed and accuracy in rapid, goal-directed movements. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[17]  Emmanuel Guigon,et al.  Computational Motor Control : Redundancy and Invariance , 2007 .

[18]  Warren B. Young,et al.  The Effect of Voluntary Effort to Influence Speed of Contraction on Strength, Muscular Power, and Hypertrophy Development , 1993 .

[19]  Lionel Rigoux,et al.  A Model of Reward- and Effort-Based Optimal Decision Making and Motor Control , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[20]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Goal-directed aiming: two components but multiple processes. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  Petros Koumoutsakos,et al.  Reducing the Time Complexity of the Derandomized Evolution Strategy with Covariance Matrix Adaptation (CMA-ES) , 2003, Evolutionary Computation.

[22]  Peter W Battaglia,et al.  Humans Trade Off Viewing Time and Movement Duration to Improve Visuomotor Accuracy in a Fast Reaching Task , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Temporal Discounting of Reward and the Cost of Time in Motor Control , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  I. MacKenzie,et al.  A note on the information-theoretic basis of Fitts' law. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  Frédéric Jean,et al.  On the Duration of Human Movement: From Self-paced to Slow/Fast Reaches up to Fitts's Law , 2017, Geometric and Numerical Foundations of Movements.

[27]  MacKenzie Is A Note on the Information-Theoretic Basis for Fitts’ Law , 1989 .

[28]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Motor Adaptation as a Process of Reoptimization , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Bruce Hoff,et al.  A model of duration in normal and perturbed reaching movement , 1994, Biological Cybernetics.

[30]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Optimal Compensation for Temporal Uncertainty in Movement Planning , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[31]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Effect of target size on spatial and temporal characteristics of a pointing movement in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  R. Plamondon On the Origin of Asymmetric Bell-Shaped Velocity Profiles in Rapid-Aimed Movements , 1991 .

[33]  M. Walton,et al.  Separate neural pathways process different decision costs , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Evidence for the Flexible Sensorimotor Strategies Predicted by Optimal Feedback Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of advance information about target location and visual feedback on movement planning and execution. , 2006, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[36]  Robert R. Bitmead,et al.  Adaptive optimal control , 1990 .

[37]  Sethu Vijayakumar,et al.  Adaptive Optimal Control for Redundantly Actuated Arms , 2008, SAB.

[38]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[39]  Emmanuel Guigon,et al.  Optimality, stochasticity, and variability in motor behavior , 2008, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[40]  Joel Myerson,et al.  Exponential Versus Hyperbolic Discounting of Delayed Outcomes: Risk and Waiting Time , 1996 .

[41]  E. R. Crossman,et al.  Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[42]  F. Jean,et al.  Why Don't We Move Slower? The Value of Time in the Neural Control of Action , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  L. Pinneo On noise in the nervous system. , 1966, Psychological review.

[44]  James L. Lyons,et al.  The Impact of Prior Knowledge about Visual Feedback on Motor Performance and Learning , 2013 .

[45]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[46]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[47]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[48]  Makoto Sato,et al.  Learning and generation of goal-directed arm reaching from scratch , 2009, Neural Networks.

[49]  S. Scott Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[50]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[51]  Okihide Hikosaka,et al.  Effects of motivational conflicts on visually elicited saccades in monkeys , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  Olivier White,et al.  Use-Dependent and Error-Based Learning of Motor Behaviors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  Tatsuya Kimura,et al.  Cerebellar complex spikes encode both destinations and errors in arm movements , 1998, Nature.

[54]  James L. Lyons,et al.  The multiple process model of goal-directed reaching revisited , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[55]  L. Green,et al.  A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[56]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[57]  E. Burdet,et al.  The duration of reaching movement is longer than predicted by minimum variance. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Impedance is modulated to meet accuracy demands during goal-directed arm movements , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  Zhong-Ping Jiang,et al.  Movement Duration, Fitts's Law, and an Infinite-Horizon Optimal Feedback Control Model for Biological Motor Systems , 2013, Neural Computation.

[61]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[62]  Ning Qian,et al.  An optimization principle for determining movement duration. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  Weiwei Li Optimal control for biological movement systems , 2006 .

[64]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .