Generalizing from the Past, Choosing the Future
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] T. Shahan. Moving Beyond Reinforcement and Response Strength , 2017, The Behavior Analyst.
[2] Valentin Dragoi,et al. Population coding of strategic variables during foraging in freely-moving macaques , 2019, bioRxiv.
[3] M Davison,et al. Performance On Concurrent Variable-interval Extinction Schedules. , 1998, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[4] M. Davison,et al. Reinforcement: food signals the time and location of future food. , 2011, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[5] M. Davison,et al. Concurrent-schedule performance in transition: changeover delays and signaled reinforcer ratios. , 2003, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[6] L. Bizo,et al. The behavioral theory of timing: Reinforcer rate determines pacemaker rate. , 1994, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[7] D. Stubbs. Temporal discrimination and a free-operant psychophysical procedure. , 1980, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[8] M. Davison,et al. Being there on time: Reinforcer effects on timing and locating. , 2020, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[9] R. Herrnstein. On the law of effect. , 1970, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[10] M. Davison,et al. Control by reinforcers across time and space: A review of recent choice research. , 2016, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[11] C. Branch,et al. What-Where-When Memory in the Rodent Odor Span Task. , 2014, Learning and motivation.
[12] O. Lazareva,et al. Relational learning in a context of transposition: a review. , 2012, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[13] M. Davison,et al. Concurrent schedules: discriminating reinforcer-ratio reversals at a fixed time after the previous reinforcer. , 2013, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[14] R J HERRNSTEIN,et al. Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. , 1961, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[15] Christopher A. Podlesnik,et al. Quantitative analysis of local-level resurgence , 2017, Learning & behavior.
[16] M. Davison,et al. Do conditional reinforcers count? , 2006, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[17] E. Wasserman,et al. A three-component model of relational responding in the transposition paradigm. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[18] Christopher A. Podlesnik,et al. Training reinforcement rates, resistance to extinction, and the role of context in reinstatement , 2016, Learning & behavior.
[19] J. Nevin,et al. Stimuli, reinforcers, and behavior: an integration. , 1999, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[20] B. Skinner,et al. The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis , 2016 .
[21] M. Davison,et al. Timing or Counting? Control by Contingency Reversals at Fixed Times or Numbers of Responses , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[22] C. L. Hull. Differential habituation to internal stimuli in the albino rat. , 1933 .
[23] Numerical reproduction in pigeons. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[24] Christian U. Krägeloh,et al. Local preference in concurrent schedules: the effects of reinforcer sequences. , 2005, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[25] Rebecca A. Sharp,et al. Lounge Layout to Facilitate Communication and Engagement in People with Dementia , 2019, Behavior analysis in practice.
[26] S. Cowie. Some weaknesses of a response-strength account of reinforcer effects , 2020, European Journal of Behavior Analysis.
[27] Kenneth W. Jacobs,et al. Coal Is Not Black, Snow Is Not White, Food Is Not a Reinforcer: The Roles of Affordances and Dispositions in the Analysis of Behavior , 2016, The Behavior Analyst.
[28] D. Blough. Steady state data and a quantitative model of operant generalization and discrimination. , 1975 .
[29] Sarah Cowie,et al. Behavioral Time Travel: Control by Past, Present, and Potential Events , 2018 .
[30] J A Nevin,et al. The analysis of behavioral momentum. , 1983, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[31] M. Davison,et al. Control by past and present stimuli depends on the discriminated reinforcer differential. , 2017, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[32] M. Bouton,et al. Context change explains resurgence after the extinction of operant behavior. , 2015, Revista mexicana de analisis de la conducta = Mexican journal of behavior analysis.
[33] K. Spence. The differential response in animals to stimuli varying within a single dimension. , 1937 .
[34] M. Bouton,et al. Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[35] A. Odum,et al. Effects of prefeeding, intercomponent-interval food, and extinction on temporal discrimination and pacemaker rate , 2006, Behavioural Processes.
[36] Lewis A. Bizo,et al. Reinforcement context and pacemaker rate in the behavioral theory of timing , 1995 .
[37] W. Estes. An experimental study of punishment. , 1944 .
[38] R. Bolles,et al. Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear , 1979 .
[39] Brad E. Pfeiffer,et al. Hippocampal place cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals , 2013, Nature.
[40] W M Baum,et al. On two types of deviation from the matching law: bias and undermatching. , 1974, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[41] R. L. Reid. The role of the reinforcer as a stimulus. , 1958, British journal of psychology.
[42] M. Davison,et al. A model for discriminating reinforcers in time and space , 2016, Behavioural Processes.
[43] Christopher A. Podlesnik,et al. Stimulus generalization and operant context renewal , 2015, Behavioural Processes.
[44] Choice, time and food: continuous cyclical changes in food probability between reinforcers. , 2014, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[45] J. Nevin,et al. Behavioral momentum and the Law of Effect , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[46] W. Baum. Rethinking reinforcement: allocation, induction, and contingency. , 2012, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[47] D. Elliffe,et al. Generalization of response patterns in a multiple peak procedure , 2018, Behavioural Processes.
[48] T. Shahan. Conditioned reinforcement and response strength. , 2010, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[49] D S Blough,et al. Recognition by the pigeon of stimuli varying in two dimensions. , 1972, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[50] J. Gibbon. Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .
[51] T. Zentall,et al. Simultaneous discrimination reversal learning in pigeons and humans: anticipatory and perseverative errors , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[52] D McCarthy,et al. Effects of mild hypoxia on perceptual-motor performance: a signal-detection approach. , 1995, Ergonomics.
[53] M. Galizio,et al. The Magic Number 70 (plus or minus 20): Variables Determining Performance in the Rodent Odor Span Task. , 2013, Learning and motivation.
[55] J. Wearden,et al. Scalar Properties in Human Timing: Conformity and Violations , 2006 .
[56] Jessica P. Stagner,et al. Episodic-like memory: Pigeons can report location pecked when unexpectedly asked , 2008, Behavioural Processes.
[57] M. Davison,et al. A model for food and stimulus changes that signal time-based contingency changes. , 2014, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[58] The differentiation of response numerosities in the pigeon. , 2007, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[59] R. Leeper. The Rôle of Motivation in Learning: A Study of the Phenomenon of Differential Motivational Control of the Utilization of Habits , 1935 .
[60] J. Rosales-Ruiz,et al. The power of one reinforcer: The effect of a single reinforcer in the context of shaping. , 2019, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.