Cognitive Endophenotypes of Dyslexia

The study investigated cognitive deficits associated with dyslexia and familial risk of dyslexia (endophenotypes) by comparing children from families with and without a history of dyslexia. Eighty-eight school-aged children were assessed on measures of phonology, language and rapid automatized naming. A series of regression analyses with family risk and dyslexia status as predictors indicated that word recall, morphology, and rapid automatized naming were associated with the deficit, whereas the two phonological measures (phoneme awareness and nonword repetition) were associated with both literacy deficits and family risk, suggesting that the phonological deficit is an endophenotype of dyslexia. Whereas the association with familial risk was similar for the two phonological measures, they differed in their relation to dyslexia status: Phoneme awareness showed a stronger association with dyslexia than risk status, whereas nonword repetition was more strongly related to the risk. The data are interpreted within the framework of multiple deficit models of dyslexia.

[1]  S. Gathercole,et al.  A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  B. Pennington,et al.  Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[3]  Charles Hulme,et al.  Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) Taps a Mechanism That Places Constraints on the Development of Early Reading Fluency , 2009, Psychological science.

[4]  Larry J. Seidman,et al.  Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Endophenotypes , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[5]  J. Wouters,et al.  Auditory processing, speech perception and phonological ability in pre-school children at high-risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal study of the auditory temporal processing theory , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  N. Freimer,et al.  Endophenotypes for psychiatric disorders: ready for primetime? , 2006, Trends in genetics : TIG.

[7]  J. Guthrie,et al.  The genetics of specific reading disability , 1976, Annals of human genetics.

[8]  Heikki Lyytinen,et al.  The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: Birth to early school age , 2004, Annals of dyslexia.

[9]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[10]  F. Castellanos,et al.  Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  F. Wijnen,et al.  Non-word repetition and literacy in Dutch children at-risk of dyslexia and children with SLI: results of the follow-up study. , 2010, Dyslexia.

[12]  R. Plomin,et al.  Generalist genes and learning disabilities. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  K. Nation,et al.  Learning to read changes children's phonological skills: evidence from a latent variable longitudinal study of reading and nonword repetition. , 2011, Developmental science.

[14]  Jan Wouters,et al.  Auditory temporal information processing in preschool children at family risk for dyslexia: Relations with phonological abilities and developing literacy skills , 2006, Brain and Language.

[15]  Criteria for evaluating behavioural interventions for neurodevelopmental disorders. , 2008, Journal of paediatrics and child health.

[16]  M. Snowling Editorial: seeking a new characterisation of learning disorders. , 2012, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[17]  H. Scarborough,et al.  Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. , 1990, Child development.

[18]  A. van der Leij,et al.  Child and parental literacy levels within families with a history of dyslexia. , 2012, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[19]  Joseph K. Torgesen,et al.  Longitudinal Studies of Phonological Processing and Reading , 1994, Journal of learning disabilities.

[20]  Richard Boada,et al.  What influences literacy outcome in children with speech sound disorder? , 2009, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[21]  K. Eklund,et al.  Brain Responses to Changes in Speech Sound Durations Differ Between Infants With and Without Familial Risk for Dyslexia , 2002, Developmental neuropsychology.

[22]  B. Pennington From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders , 2006, Cognition.

[23]  M. Rutter,et al.  Genes and behavior: nature-nurture interplay explained , 2006, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[24]  L. Waites,et al.  Definition of dyslexia. , 1968, Canadian Medical Association journal.

[25]  C. McBride-Chang,et al.  Towards Early Identification of Dyslexia in Chinese Preschool Children: A Study on Reading and Cognitive Profile in Children with Genetic Risk of Dyslexia in Hong Kong , 2008 .

[26]  George K. Georgiou,et al.  Naming Speed and Reading: From Prediction to Instruction , 2010 .

[27]  D. Skuse Endophenotypes and child psychiatry , 2001, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[28]  L. C. Bidwell,et al.  Testing for Neuropsychological Endophenotypes in Siblings Discordant for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[29]  A. Monk,et al.  Learning to read: the role of short-term memory and phonological skills. , 1994, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[30]  I. Zink,et al.  Who Is At Risk for Dyslexia? Phonological Processing in Five-to Seven-Year-Old Dutch-Speaking Children With SLI , 2010 .

[31]  Uta Frith,et al.  Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. , 2003, Child development.

[32]  Carsten Elbro,et al.  Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten : The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items , 1998 .

[33]  E. Gerrits,et al.  Early language development of children at familial risk of dyslexia: speech perception and production. , 2009, Journal of communication disorders.

[34]  Julia M. Carroll,et al.  Language and phonological skills in children at high risk of reading difficulties. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[35]  H. Wimmer,et al.  The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. , 2000 .

[36]  B. Pennington,et al.  Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. , 2001, Child development.

[37]  T. Campbell,et al.  Nonword repetition and child language impairment. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[38]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. , 1987 .

[39]  T. Little,et al.  The relationship between phonological awareness and reading: implications for the assessment of phonological awareness. , 2005, Language, speech, and hearing services in schools.

[40]  P. Heaton,et al.  Developmental Disorders of Language, Learning and Cognition , 2012 .

[41]  Charles Hulme,et al.  Annual Research Review: The nature and classification of reading disorders – a commentary on proposals for DSM-5 , 2011, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[42]  J B McKendry,et al.  Hemodynamic observations during closed-chest cardiac massage. , 1968, Canadian Medical Association journal.

[43]  F. Ramus,et al.  Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  H. Lyytinen,et al.  Emerging phonological awareness differentiates children with and without familial risk for dyslexia after controlling for general language skills , 2004, Annals of dyslexia.

[45]  Simpson W. L. Wong,et al.  Early predictors of dyslexia in Chinese children: familial history of dyslexia, language delay, and cognitive profiles. , 2011, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[46]  P. F. Jong,et al.  Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography. , 2003 .

[47]  M. Snowling,et al.  Specific Disorders and Broader Phenotypes: The Case of Dyslexia , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[48]  B. Byrne,et al.  Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. , 2012, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[49]  Heikki Lyytinen,et al.  Speech Perception of Infants With High Familial Risk for Dyslexia Differ at the Age of 6 Months , 2003, Developmental neuropsychology.

[50]  Hollis S. Scarborough,et al.  Antecedents to reading disability: Preschool language development and literacy experiences of children from dyslexic families , 1991 .

[51]  T. Q. Irigaray,et al.  Intellectual abilities in Alzheimer's disease patients: Contributions from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) , 2010 .

[52]  D. Bishop,et al.  Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: same or different? , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[53]  S. Valdois,et al.  Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis , 2007, Cognition.

[54]  U. Frith,et al.  Precursors of literacy delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia. , 2000, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[55]  J. Buitelaar,et al.  Relationship between endophenotype and phenotype in ADHD , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[56]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence , 2014 .

[57]  D. Bishop,et al.  Children Who Read Words Accurately Despite Language Impairment: Who Are They and How Do They Do It? , 2009, Child development.

[58]  I. Gottesman,et al.  The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[59]  Yonata Levy,et al.  Research review: crossing syndrome boundaries in the search for brain endophenotypes. , 2009, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.