Individual laboratory-measured discount rates predict field behavior

We estimate discount rates of 555 subjects using a laboratory task and find that these individual discount rates predict inter-individual variation in field behaviors (e.g., exercise, BMI, smoking). The correlation between the discount rate and each field behavior is small: none exceeds 0.28 and many are near 0. However, the discount rate has at least as much predictive power as any variable in our dataset (e.g., sex, age, education). The correlation between the discount rate and field behavior rises when field behaviors are aggregated: these correlations range from 0.09–0.38. We present a model that explains why specific intertemporal choice behaviors are only weakly correlated with discount rates, even though discount rates robustly predict aggregates of intertemporal decisions.

[1]  Miles S. Kimball,et al.  Preference Parameters and Behavioral Heterogeneity: An Experimental Approach in the Health and Retirement Survey , 1995 .

[2]  E. Gilpin,et al.  Do favorite movie stars influence adolescent smoking initiation? , 2004, American journal of public health.

[3]  A. Jensen,et al.  The g factor , 1996, Nature.

[4]  K. Kirby,et al.  Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  Aldo Rustichini,et al.  Cognitive Skills Explain Economic Preferences, Strategic Behavior, and Job Attachment , 2008, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[6]  E. Gilpin,et al.  Do movie stars encourage adolescents to start smoking? Evidence from California. , 1999, Preventive medicine.

[7]  D. Bors,et al.  Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices: Norms for First-Year University Students and the Development of a Short Form , 1998 .

[8]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Anomalies in Intertemporal Choice: Evidence and an Interpretation , 1992 .

[9]  M. Dixon,et al.  Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. , 2003, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[10]  J. E. Mazur An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. , 1987 .

[11]  N. Petry,et al.  Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls , 2001, Psychopharmacology.

[12]  D. Cross,et al.  Subjective probability and delay. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[13]  Christopher F. Chabris,et al.  Cognitive and Neurobiological Mechanisms of the Law of General Intelligence , 2006 .

[14]  Noah A. Shamosh,et al.  Delay Discounting and Intelligence: A Meta-Analysis. , 2008 .

[15]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of Delayed Rewards: A Life-Span Comparison , 1994 .

[16]  B. Reynolds A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to drug use and gambling , 2006, Behavioural pharmacology.

[17]  D. Cohen Does IQ Matter , 1972 .

[18]  M. Grossman On the Concept of Health Capital and the Demand for Health , 1972, Journal of Political Economy.

[19]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[20]  L. Blume,et al.  The New Palgrave Dictionary of Economics, 2nd edition , 2008 .

[21]  Aldo Rustichini,et al.  Cognitive skills affect economic preferences, strategic behavior, and job attachment , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  W. Swann,et al.  Personality Psychology’s Comeback and Its Emerging Symbiosis With Social Psychology , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[23]  L. Ross,et al.  The Person and the Situation: Perspectives of Social Psychology , 1991 .

[24]  David I. Laibson,et al.  Golden Eggs and Hyperbolic Discounting , 1997 .

[25]  E S Geller,et al.  Behavioral interventions. , 1988, Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine.

[26]  Stacey L. Small,et al.  Impulsivity, self‐control, and delay discounting in persons with acquired brain injury , 2005 .

[27]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[28]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[29]  Till Sprenger,et al.  The runner's high: opioidergic mechanisms in the human brain. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  A. Odum,et al.  Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[31]  References , 1971 .

[32]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[33]  R. H. Strotz The Empirical Implications of a Utility Tree , 1957 .

[34]  B R Rosen,et al.  The Multi-Source Interference Task: validation study with fMRI in individual subjects , 2003, Molecular Psychiatry.

[35]  J. Hellige,et al.  Categorization versus distance: Hemispheric differences for processing spatial information , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[36]  M. McCloskey,et al.  IQ and nonplanning impulsivity are independently associated with delay discounting in middle-aged adults , 2007 .

[37]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[38]  C. Chabris,et al.  Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  W. Bickel,et al.  Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[40]  Stephen M Kosslyn,et al.  The minicog rapid assessment battery: developing a "blood pressure cuff for the mind". , 2005, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[41]  J. Stevens Intertemporal Choice , 2013 .

[42]  C. F. Bond,et al.  One Hundred Years of Social Psychology Quantitatively Described , 2003 .