Education and Cognitive Development: A Natural Experiment

Using an arbitrary cutoff date, school districts regulate which children will begin school. This "natural experiment" was used to examine effects of age- and schooling-related influences on memory and 3 levels of phonological segmentation in children who just made vs. missed the cutoff. Group comparisons over time permitted assessment of schooling influences and Age X Experience interactions. Short-term memory was enhanced by grade one schooling, with no evidence of an Age X Schooling interaction. For phonological segmentation, both schooling- and age-related influences appeared, with unique patterns for each level of segmentation. The cutoff method proved sensitive to important changes in cognitive skills during this age period. In many Western societies, the period from late preschool through early elementary school is characterized by widespread, perhaps qualitative, shifts in children's cognitive, social, and moral functioning. Collectively, the changes have been referred to as the 5-7 shift (White, 1965). In the intellectual realm, children's thinking has been described as becoming more logical (Piaget, 1960) and more abstract (Rogoff, 1981); memory performance improves as rehearsal and organizational strategies become more active (Bjorklund, 1987; Ornstein & Naus, 1978); language skills become more reflective and refined (Read & Schreiber, 1982); and increased metacognitive skills yield greater planning, control, and evaluation over the sequencing of cognitive acts (Case, 1985; White, 1965). Although their uniqueness may be challenged, the existence of major changes between ages 5 and 7 is not in dispute. However, the relative contribution of maturation versus experience (e.g., schooling) has not been resolved. For most of the past 2 decades, the maturationalist view has dominated theory and research. The work of Piaget and others (Case, 1985; Kagan, 1984) has emphasized regular, uniform, and seemingly universal changes in thinking across the age range that appeared internally driven and controlled by a maturational timetable. Nevertheless, recent findings have revealed that specific learning experiences may play a crucial role in the growth of selected cognitive skills. First, cross-cultural research has documented major differences between schooled and unschooled children in growth of perceptual and memory skills, concept development,

[1]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Toward a model of text comprehension and production. , 1978 .

[2]  G. E. MacKinnon,et al.  Reading Research Advances in Theory and Practice , 1985 .

[3]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? , 1979, Cognition.

[4]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. , 1987 .

[5]  M. Chi Knowledge structures and memory development. , 1978 .

[6]  Jeffrey K. Liker,et al.  A day at the races: A study of IQ, expertise, and cognitive complexity. , 1986 .

[7]  Peter A. Ornstein,et al.  Memory development in children , 2014 .

[8]  M. A. Merrill,et al.  Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale , 1972 .

[9]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  How Orthography Alters Spoken Language Competencies in Children Learning to Read and Spell , 1984 .

[10]  Eric Wanner,et al.  Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .

[11]  John B. Carroll,et al.  The American Heritage Word Frequency Book , 1971 .

[12]  K. Rayner,et al.  The psychology of reading , 1989 .

[13]  M A Just,et al.  A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. , 1980, Psychological review.

[14]  S. Cahan,et al.  Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. , 1989, Child development.

[15]  M. Chi,et al.  Network representation of a child's dinosaur knowledge. , 1983 .

[16]  K. Fischer A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. , 1980 .

[17]  F. Morrison,et al.  Nature-nurture in the classroom: entrance age, school readiness, and learning in children. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[18]  P. Baltes,et al.  Cohort effects in cognitive development of children as revealed by cross-sectional sequences. , 1969 .

[19]  Why Short Subjects Are Harder to Find Than Long Ones. Technical Report No. 466. , 1978 .

[20]  J. Downing,et al.  Language awareness and learning to read , 1984 .

[21]  J. Piaget The Psychology Of Intelligence , 1951 .

[22]  Lynette Bradley,et al.  Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling , 1985 .

[23]  J. Stigler,et al.  Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children. , 1986, Science.

[24]  J. Hagen,et al.  Prompting and Rehearsal in Short-Term Memory. , 1973 .

[25]  Robbie Case,et al.  Intellectual development : birth to adulthood , 1985 .

[26]  R. R. Hunt,et al.  Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology , 1993 .

[27]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  How Inferences about Novel Domain-Related Concepts Can Be Constrained by Structured Knowledge. , 1989 .

[28]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children's memory: An interpretive review , 1987 .

[29]  Jerome Kagan,et al.  The nature of the child , 1986 .

[30]  S. White,et al.  Evidence for a Hierarchical Arrangement of Learning Processes , 1965 .

[31]  Peter A. Ornstein,et al.  Rehearsal processes in children's memory , 1973 .

[32]  C. Varnhagen,et al.  Age and Schooling Effects in Story Recall and Story Production. , 1994 .

[33]  S. Meisels Doing Harm by Doing Good: latrogenic Effects of Early Childhood Enrollment and Promotion Policies , 1992 .

[34]  U. Goswami,et al.  Phonological Skills and Learning to Read , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  The mnemonic value of orthography among beginning readers. , 1979 .

[36]  J. Bisanz,et al.  Effects of age and schooling on the acquisition of elementary quantitative skills. , 1995 .

[37]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Intelligence, Cognitive Skills, and Early Reading Progress. , 1984 .

[38]  R. Treiman Onsets and rimes as units of spoken syllables: evidence from children. , 1985, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[39]  Sorel Cahan,et al.  Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. , 1989 .

[40]  Sorel Cahan,et al.  A Between-Grade-Levels Approach to the Investigation of the Absolute Effects of Schooling on Achievement , 1987 .

[41]  R. Siegler Children's Thinking : What Develops? , 1978 .

[42]  Shlomo Bentin,et al.  The Effects of Aging and First Grade Schooling on the Development of Phonological Awareness , 1991 .

[43]  J. Stigler,et al.  Contexts of achievement: a study of American, Chinese, and Japanese children. , 1990, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[44]  Barbara J. Fox,et al.  Phonemic Analysis and Synthesis as Word-Attack Skills. , 1976 .

[45]  Earl C. Butterfield,et al.  Learning strategies as determinants of memory deficiencies , 1971 .

[46]  R. Treiman,et al.  Phonemic-analysis training helps children benefit from spelling-sound rules , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[47]  Judith A. Bowey,et al.  Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction , 1991, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[48]  C. Read,et al.  The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing , 1986, Cognition.

[49]  D. Share,et al.  Sources of Individual Differences in Reading Acquisition. , 1984 .

[50]  F. Ferreira,et al.  Children's metalinguistic knowledge of syntactic constituents: Effects of age and schooling. , 1994 .