Orientation in Virtual Reality Does Not Fully Measure Up to the Real-World

Adult participants learned to reorient to a specific corner inside either a real or virtual rectangular room containing a distinct featural object in each corner. Participants in the virtual-reality (VR) condition experienced an immersive virtual version of the physical room using a head-mounted display (HMD) and customized manual wheelchair to provide self-movement. Following a disorientation procedure, people could reorient by using either the geometry of the room and/or the distinct features in the corners. Test trials in which the different spatial cues were manipulated revealed participants encoded features and geometry in both the real and VR rooms. However, participants in the VR room showed less facility with using geometry. Our results suggest caution must be taken when interpreting the nuances of spatial cue use in virtual environments. Reduced reliability of geometric cues in VR environments may result in greater reliance on feature cues than would normally be expected under similar real-world conditions.

[1]  William B. Thompson,et al.  HMD calibration and its effects on distance judgments , 2008, APGV '08.

[2]  D R Proffitt,et al.  Comparing depth from motion with depth from binocular disparity. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. , 2003 .

[4]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[6]  R. Astur,et al.  Sex differences and correlations in a virtual Morris water task, a virtual radial arm maze, and mental rotation , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  K. Bodily,et al.  Evidence consistent with the multiple-bearings hypothesis from human virtual landmark-based navigation , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[8]  Ekaterina P. Volkova,et al.  The effect of landmark and body-based sensory information on route knowledge , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[9]  Jennifer E Sutton,et al.  Spinning in the scanner: neural correlates of virtual reorientation. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  Robert S. Astur,et al.  Human sex differences in solving a virtual navigation problem , 2016, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  HMD calibration and its effects on distance judgments , 2009, ACM Trans. Appl. Percept..

[12]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[13]  Joanne Lloyd,et al.  Equivalence of Real-World and Virtual-Reality Route Learning: A Pilot Study , 2009, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[14]  Debbie M Kelly,et al.  Reorienting in images of a three-dimensional environment. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Jodie M. Plumert,et al.  Distance perception in real and virtual environments , 2004, APGV '04.

[16]  B. Brooks Route learning in a case of amnesia : A preliminary investigation into the efficacy of training in a virtual environment , 1999 .

[17]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  The Influence of Restricted Viewing Conditions on Egocentric Distance Perception: Implications for Real and Virtual Indoor Environments , 2005, Perception.

[18]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Limited Field of View of Head-Mounted Displays Is Not the Cause of Distance Underestimation in Virtual Environments , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[19]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  Marcia L Spetch,et al.  Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: I. Do adults encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room? , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.

[21]  Bob G. Witmer,et al.  Judging Perceived and Traversed Distance in Virtual Environments , 1998, Presence.

[22]  Zahra Moussavi,et al.  Design and Application of a Novel Virtual Reality Navigational Technology (VRNChair) , 2014, Journal of experimental neuroscience.

[23]  Ahmad Byagowi,et al.  Introducing a new age-and-cognition-sensitive measurement for assessing spatial orientation using a landmark-less virtual reality navigational task , 2017, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[24]  Ken Cheng,et al.  25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: a current theoretical perspective , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[25]  Michiel H G Claessen,et al.  A Direct Comparison of Real-World and Virtual Navigation Performance in Chronic Stroke Patients , 2016, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[26]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[27]  Albert Postma,et al.  Distance comparisons in virtual reality: effects of path, context, and age , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[28]  Kent D. Bodily,et al.  Is surface-based orientation influenced by a proportional relationship of shape parameters? , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[29]  Paul J. White,et al.  Effect of viewing mode on pathfinding in immersive Virtual Reality , 2015, 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[30]  Debbie M. Kelly,et al.  Reorienting in Virtual 3D Environments: Do Adult Humans Use Principal Axes, Medial Axes or Local Geometry? , 2013, PloS one.

[31]  P. Dudchenko The hippocampus as a cognitive map , 2010 .