Through the (Thin-Slice) Looking Glass: An Initial Look at Rapport and Co-Construction Within Peer Collaboration

Within peer collaboration, both cognitive and social phenomena have been identified as important components for success, though little is known about the relationship between these factors. In this work, we examined math collaboration discourse between 11 4t h grade dyads in 30-second slices to investigate the relationship between rapport state and reasoning state. Prior to collaboration, students watched one of three instructional videos modeling either domain knowledge, collaborate reasoning, or both. There was no impact of video type on student talk behaviors, nor posttest scores. However, we found a correlation between high rapport states and strong reasoning states, as well as a marginal effect of more co-constructive reasoning leading to improved posttest scores. This work demonstrates that students’ rapport states may play a role in students’ reasoning states, and thus calls for a deeper investigation within the CSCL community about the role of rapport in peer learning.

[1]  Nikol Rummel,et al.  Learning to collaborate while being scripted or by observing a model , 2009, Int. J. Comput. Support. Collab. Learn..

[2]  Richard Cox,et al.  Vicarious learning from educational dialogue , 1999, CSCL.

[3]  Carolyn Penstein Rosé,et al.  Using transactivity in conversation for summarization of educational dialogue , 2007, SLaTE.

[4]  Amy Ogan,et al.  The Effect of Friendship and Tutoring Roles on Reciprocal Peer Tutoring Strategies , 2016, ITS.

[5]  Helen R. Quinn,et al.  Science and Language for English Language Learners in Relation to Next Generation Science Standards and with Implications for Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts and Mathematics , 2013 .

[6]  Carol K. K. Chan,et al.  Information Processing Approaches to Collaborative Learning , 2013 .

[7]  P. Decker,et al.  Behavior Modeling Training: Principles and Applications , 1985 .

[8]  L. Tickle-Degnen,et al.  The Nature of Rapport and Its Nonverbal Correlates , 1990 .

[9]  Pierre Dillenbourg,et al.  Over-scripting CSCL: The risks of blending collaborative learning with instructional design , 2002 .

[10]  F. Fischer,et al.  Epistemic and social scripts in computer–supported collaborative learning , 2005 .

[11]  B. Brown,et al.  “It isn't no slang that can be said about this stuff”: Language, identity, and appropriating science discourse , 2006 .

[12]  Y. Lou,et al.  Small Group and Individual Learning with Technology: A Meta-Analysis , 2001 .

[13]  Amy Ogan,et al.  Rudeness and Rapport: Insults and Learning Gains in Peer Tutoring , 2012, ITS.

[14]  Jan M. Zottmann,et al.  Collaboration Scripts in Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning , 2013 .

[15]  J. Gee Identity as an analytic lens for research in education , 2000 .

[16]  Nora A. Murphy,et al.  Using Thin Slices for Behavioral Coding , 2005 .

[17]  F. Fischer,et al.  Collaboration Scripts – A Conceptual Analysis , 2006 .

[18]  M. Chi,et al.  Dialogue Patterns in Peer Collaboration That Promote Learning , 2015 .

[19]  M. Chi,et al.  The ICAP Framework: Linking Cognitive Engagement to Active Learning Outcomes , 2014 .

[20]  M. Azmitia,et al.  Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning , 1993 .

[21]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Observing Tutorial Dialogues Collaboratively: Insights About Human Tutoring Effectiveness From Vicarious Learning , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[22]  S. Michaels,et al.  Deliberative Discourse Idealized and Realized: Accountable Talk in the Classroom and in Civic Life , 2008 .

[23]  Justine Cassell,et al.  We Click, We Align, We Learn: Impact of Influence and Convergence Processes on Student Learning and Rapport Building , 2015, INTERPERSONAL@ICMI.