Amphetamine, conditioned stimulus, and nondebilitating preshock effects on activity and avoidance: further evidence for interactions between associative and nonassociative changes.

[1]  S. Maier,et al.  Coping and the stress-induced potentiation of stimulant stereotypy in the rat. , 1983, Science.

[2]  M. Fanselow The postshock activity burst , 1982 .

[3]  S. Maier,et al.  Dissociation of long-term analgesia and the shuttle box escape deficit caused by inescapable shock. , 1982, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[4]  H. Anisman,et al.  Effects of dopamine receptor blockade on avoidance performance: assessment of effects on cue-shock and response-outcome associations. , 1982, Behavioral and neural biology.

[5]  R. Rosellini,et al.  Evidence for a serotonergic mechanism of the learned helplessness phenomenon , 1982, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[6]  L. Kokkinidis,et al.  Stress-induced facilitation of acoustic startle after d-amphetamine administration , 1982, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[7]  S. Maier,et al.  The nature of the activity deficit produced by inescapable shock , 1982 .

[8]  H. Anisman,et al.  Stimulus change influences escape performance: Deficits induced by uncontrollable stress and by haloperidol , 1982, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[9]  L. Watkins,et al.  Organization of endogenous opiate and nonopiate pain control systems. , 1982, Science.

[10]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Conditioned fear-induced opiate analgesia on the Formalin test: Evidence for two aversive motivational systems , 1982 .

[11]  J. L. Martínez,et al.  Modulation of memory storage by treatments affecting peripheral catecholamines , 1982 .

[12]  F. Petty,et al.  Specificity of the learned helplessness model of depression , 1982, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[13]  Z. Amit,et al.  Stress-induced analgesia: A performance deficit or a change in pain responsiveness? , 1982, Behavioral and neural biology.

[14]  S. Maier,et al.  Controllability, coping behavior, and stress-induced analgesia in the rat , 1982, Pain.

[15]  J. Weiss,et al.  Behavioral depression produced by an uncontrollable stressor: Relationship to norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin levels in various regions of rat brain , 1981, Brain Research Reviews.

[16]  S. Maier,et al.  Long-term stress-induced analgesia and activation of the opiate system. , 1981, Science.

[17]  H. Anisman,et al.  Social housing conditions influence escape deficits produced by uncontrollable stress: assessment of the contribution of norepinephrine. , 1981, Behavioral and neural biology.

[18]  R. L. Singhal,et al.  Effects of nortriptyline treatment on learned helplessness in the rat , 1981, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[19]  G. Coover,et al.  Motor debilitation, short- and long-term shuttlebox deficits, and brain monoamine changes following footshock pretreatment in rats , 1981 .

[20]  D. Cogan,et al.  Learned helplessness: Now you see it, now you don’t , 1981 .

[21]  S. Maier,et al.  Therapy and immunization of long-term analgesia in rats , 1981 .

[22]  R. Lubow,et al.  Performance Decrement Following Shock Preexposure: The Effect of Number and Duration of Escapable and Inescapable Shocks , 1981 .

[23]  C. Crowell,et al.  Influence of duration and number of inescapable shocks on intrashock activity and subsequent interference effects , 1981 .

[24]  H. Anisman,et al.  Dissociation of antinociception and escape deficits induced by stress in mice. , 1980, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[25]  S. Maier,et al.  Opiate antagonists and long-term analgesic reaction induced by inescapable shock in rats. , 1980 .

[26]  D. E. Mcallister,et al.  Escape-from-fear performance as affected by handling method and an additional CS-shock treatment , 1980 .

[27]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Attenuation of amphetamine-induced enhancement of learning by adrenal demedullation , 1980, Brain Research.

[28]  S. Maier,et al.  Conditioned analgesia in the rat , 1980 .

[29]  R. Bolles,et al.  Conditioned freezing in the rat as a function of shock intensity and CS modality , 1980 .

[30]  W. Chance Autoanalgesia: Opiate and non-opiate mechanisms , 1980, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[31]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Central and peripheral actions of amphetamine on memory storage , 1980, Brain Research.

[32]  S. Antelman,et al.  Interchangeability of stress and amphetamine in sensitization. , 1980, Science.

[33]  M. McDaniel,et al.  The time course of learned helplessness, inactivity, and nociceptive deficits in rats , 1979 .

[34]  F. Henn,et al.  A neuropharmacologically-relevant animal model of depression , 1979, Neuropharmacology.

[35]  S. Maier,et al.  Long-term analgesic effects of inescapable shock and learned helplessness. , 1979, Science.

[36]  H. Anisman,et al.  Catecholamine depletion in mice upon reexposure to stress: mediation of the escape deficits produced by inescapable shock. , 1979, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[37]  C. Crowell,et al.  Behavior during shock exposure as a determinant of subsequent interference with shuttle box escape-avoidance learning in the rat , 1979 .

[38]  W. Beatty Failure to observe learned helplessness in rats exposed to inescapable footshock , 1979 .

[39]  H. Anisman,et al.  Escape performance after inescapable shock in selectively bred lines of mice: response maintenance and catecholamine activity. , 1979, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[40]  H. Anisman,et al.  Shock-induced locomotor excitation following acute and chronic amphetamine treatment , 1979, Neuropharmacology.

[41]  H. Anisman,et al.  Escape Performance Following Exposure to Inescapable Shock: Deficits in Motor Response Maintenance. , 1978 .

[42]  K. Hollis,et al.  Interference with avoidance behavior as a function of qualitative properties of inescapable shocks , 1978 .

[43]  Steven F. Maier,et al.  Exposure to inescapable shock produces both activity and associative deficits in the rat , 1978 .

[44]  H. Anisman Time-dependent changes in activity, reactivity, and responsivity during shock: effects of cholinergic and catecholaminergic manipulations. , 1977, Behavioral biology.

[45]  M. Feldman The effects of preexposure to a warning or a safety signal on the acquisition of a two-way avoidance response in rats , 1977 .

[46]  M. Frontali,et al.  A pharmacological analysis of processes underlying differential responding: a review and further experiments with scopolamine, amphetamine, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD-25), chlordiazepoxide, physostigmine, and chlorpromazine. , 1976, Behavioral biology.

[47]  C. Crowell,et al.  Different intensities of unsignalled inescapable shock treatments as determinants of non-shock-motivated open field behavior: A resolution of disparate results , 1976, Physiology & Behavior.

[48]  J. Weiss,et al.  Long-term and transitory interference effects. , 1976 .

[49]  J. Weiss,et al.  Long-term interference effect: An alternative to learned helplessness. , 1976 .

[50]  A. G. Baker Learned Irrelevance and Learned Helplessness: Rats Learn that Stimuli, Reinforcers, and Responses are Uncorrelated. , 1976 .

[51]  M. Seligman,et al.  Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. , 1976 .

[52]  H. Anisman,et al.  Central effects of scopolamine and (+)-amphetamine on locomotor activity: Interaction with strain and stress variables , 1975, Neuropharmacology.

[53]  N. Miller,et al.  Effects of Chronic Exposure to Stressors on Avoidance‐Escape Behavior and on Brain Norepinephrine* , 1975, Psychosomatic medicine.

[54]  J. Weiss,et al.  Effects of Acute Exposure to Stressors on Subsequent Avoidance‐Escape Behavior* , 1975, Psychosomatic medicine.

[55]  N. Miller,et al.  Monamines as Mediators of Avoidance‐Escape Behavior* , 1975, Psychosomatic medicine.

[56]  W. Redmon,et al.  Factors affecting the measurement of classically conditioned fear in rats following exposure to escapable versus inescapable signaled shock. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[57]  H. Anisman,et al.  Effects of d-amphetamine and scopolamine on activity before and after shock in three mouse strains , 1975, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[58]  H. Anisman Time-dependent variations in aversively motivated behaviors: nonassociative effects of cholinergic and catecholaminergic activity. , 1975, Psychological review.

[59]  S. Maier,et al.  Failure to learn to escape by rats previously exposed to inescapable shock is partly produced by associative interference. , 1975 .

[60]  M. Seligman,et al.  Learned helplessness in the rat. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[61]  M. Seligman,et al.  Learned helplessness in the rat: time course, immunization, and reversibility. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[62]  M. Frontali,et al.  Stimulus nonequivalences in go/no-go avoidance discriminations: Sensory, drive, and response factors , 1974 .

[63]  D. Wahlsten,et al.  Interacting effects of handling and d-amphetamine on avoidance learning. , 1974, Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior.

[64]  A. Black,et al.  The effects of restraint and noncontingent preshock on subsequent escape learning in the rat , 1974 .

[65]  S. Maier,et al.  Failure to learn to escape in rats previously exposed to inescapable shock depends on nature of escape response. , 1973 .

[66]  H. Anisman Effects of response restriction during exposure to inescapable shock upon subsequent one-way and shuttle-avoidance performance in rats. , 1973, Canadian journal of psychology.

[67]  H. Anisman,et al.  Effects of inescapable shock on subsequent avoidance performance: role of response repertoire changes. , 1973, Behavioral biology.

[68]  S. Levine,et al.  Dissociation of open-field behavior and pituitary-adrenal function , 1973 .

[69]  G. Bignami,et al.  An investigation on the nature of continuous avoidance deficits: differential response to chlordiazepoxide treatment. , 1973, Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior.

[70]  H. Anisman,et al.  Footshock-produced excitation and inhibition of activity in rats , 1973 .

[71]  S. Levine,et al.  Physiological and behavioral effects of prior aversive stimulation (preshock) in the rat. , 1973, Physiology & behavior.

[72]  M. Frontali,et al.  Go-no go avoidance discriminations in rats with simple “go” and compound “no go” signals: Stimulus modality and stimulus intensity , 1973 .

[73]  T. A. Looney,et al.  Retardation of jump-up escape responding in rats pretreated with different frequencies of noncontingent electric shock. , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[74]  R. Bauer,et al.  Twenty-four-hour proactive facilitation of avoidance and discrimination learning in rats by d-amphetamine. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[75]  M. Frontali,et al.  Central cholinergic blockade and two-way avoidance acquisition: the role of response disinhibition. , 1971, Physiology & behavior.

[76]  H. Anisman,et al.  Effects of methamphetamine and shock duration during inescapable shock exposure on subsequent active and passive avoidance. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[77]  G. Cicala,et al.  The effects of prefear conditioning shock intensity on initial shuttle response rate , 1971 .

[78]  R. Porsolt,et al.  Persistence of Dose Related Behaviour in Mice , 1971, Nature.

[79]  G. A. Heise,et al.  Stimulant action of d-amphetamine in relation to test compartment dimensions and behavioral measure. , 1971, Neuropharmacology.

[80]  H. Anisman Passive-Avoidance Learning in Mice following Methamphetamine or Nembutal Injection during Inescapable Exposure to Shock , 1971 .

[81]  G. Cicala,et al.  Reliable immobility and activity enhancement are produced by a fear CS , 1971 .

[82]  D. E. Mcallister,et al.  The inverse relationship between shock intensity and shuttle-box avoidance learning in rats: A reinforcement explanation. , 1971 .

[83]  G. Bignami,et al.  Facilitation and impairment of avoidance responding by phenobarbital sodium, chlordiazepoxide and diazepam--the role of performance base lines. , 1971, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[84]  E. E. Krieckhaus,et al.  Effects of fear conditioning on subsequent avoidance behavior and movement. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[85]  D. C. Anderson,et al.  Variations in unsignaled inescapable preshock as determinants of responses to punishment. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[86]  Black Ah,et al.  Effects of preshock on subsequent avoidance conditioning. , 1967 .

[87]  G. Mogenson,et al.  Effects of Fear Conditioning on Avoidance Learning , 1963 .

[88]  Seymour Geisser,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1963 .

[89]  J. E. Sherman,et al.  The opioid/nonopioid nature of stress-induced analgesia and learned helplessness. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[90]  R. Bolles,et al.  Endorphins and behavior. , 1982, Annual review of psychology.

[91]  H. Anisman,et al.  Differential effects of inescapable shock on escape performance and discrimination learning in a water escape task. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[92]  S. Maier,et al.  Learned Helplessness: All of us were Right (And Wrong): Inescapable Shock has Multiple Effects , 1979 .

[93]  G. Bignami Pharmacological evidence on the specialization of CNS mechanisms responsible for motor act inhibition by aversive events. , 1979, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[94]  M. Seligman,et al.  On the Cognitive Component of Learned Helplessness and Depression1 , 1979 .

[95]  G. Bignami,et al.  Cholinergic Mechanisms and Aversively Motivated Behaviors , 1978 .

[96]  G. Bignami Effects of Neuroleptics, Ethanol, Hypnotic-Sedatives, Tranquilizers, Narcotics, and Minor Stimulants in Aversive Paradigms , 1978 .

[97]  H. Anisman,et al.  Monoamines and Aversively Motivated Behaviors , 1978 .

[98]  A. Black Comments on "Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence." by Maier and Seligman. , 1977 .

[99]  G. Bignami,et al.  Nonassociative explanations of behavioral changes induced by central cholinergic drugs. , 1976, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[100]  G. Bignami Behavioral pharmacology and toxicology. , 1976, Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology.

[101]  L. Pohorecky,et al.  Coping Behavior and Neurochemical Changes An Alternative Explanation for the Original "Learned Helplessness" Experiments , 1976 .

[102]  R. Katzev,et al.  Escape/avoidance responding in rats depends on strain and number of inescapable preshocks. , 1975 .

[103]  K. Weiss,et al.  Latent inhibition: a review and a new hypothesis. , 1974, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[104]  D. E. Mcallister,et al.  Magnitude and shift of reward in instrumental aversive learning in rats. , 1972 .

[105]  H. Anisman,et al.  Facilitative and disruptive effects of prior exposure to shock of subsequent avoidance performance. , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[106]  F. Masterson,et al.  Role of initial response rate in avoidance learning by rats. , 1971 .

[107]  Martin E. P. Seligman,et al.  Unpredictable and Uncontrollable Aversive Events , 1971 .

[108]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Effect of amphetamine, nicotine and hexamethonium on performance of a conditioned response during acquisition and retention trials. , 1970, Pharmacology.

[109]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  The effect of amphetamine, nicotine and atropine on pseudoconditioned responses of rats. , 1970, Pharmacology.

[110]  K. Zieliński,et al.  THE EFFECT OF PRETRAINING ON THE ACQUISITION AND EXTINCTION OF AVOIDANCE REFLEX. , 1964, Acta biologiae experimentalis.

[111]  K. Brookshire,et al.  Residua of shock-trauma in the white rat: A three factor theory. , 1961 .