Postadaptation orientation discrimination.

An orientational difference of only 0.3-0.5 deg can be discriminated between two gratings or two lines, although psychophysical channels and cortical cells both have comparatively broad orientation bandwidths of 10-25 deg. One proposed explanation for the fineness of orientation discrimination is that, while detection is determined by the most excited orientation-tuned neural elements, superthreshold orientation discrimination is determined by difference signals between these elements [Westheimer et al., J. Opt. Soc. Am. 66, 332 (1976)]. This implies that, if stimulus orientation is changed slightly, the most important elements for discriminating this change will be those whose relative activity changes most, even though the excitation of these elements may be comparatively weak. In accord with this prediction, we found that adapting to a high-contrast grating degraded discrimination for test gratings inclined at about 10-20 deg to the adapting grating while having little effect on the detection of these inclined gratings. For test gratings parallel to the adapting grating, discrimination was improved, but detection was degraded. Either an opponent-process or a line-element model can account for these effects of adaptation. An opponent model can also explain our findings that subjects do not confound orientation change with contrast change and that suprathreshold orientation discrimination is almost independent of contrast, varying by only +/- 10% from about 3 to about 25 times contrast threshold. A discrimination model must incorporate reliable storage of spatial frequency, because discrimination was not affected by increasing the interval between grating presentations from 1 to 10 sec.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)

[1]  M. Vernon The perception of inclined lines. , 1934 .

[2]  J. Gibson,et al.  Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. Quantitative studies , 1937 .

[3]  D. P. Andrews Perception of Contours in the Central Fovea , 1965, Nature.

[4]  F. Campbell,et al.  Orientational selectivity of the human visual system , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  D. P. Andrews Perception of contour orientation in the central fovea part II. Spatial integration , 1967 .

[6]  D. P. Andrews Perception of contour orientation in the central fovea part I: Short lines , 1967 .

[7]  G. F. Cooper,et al.  The angular selectivity of visual cortical cells to moving gratings , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  A. S. Gilinsky Orientation-specific effects of patterns of adapting light on visual acuity. , 1968, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[9]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  J. Robson,et al.  Application of fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  C Blakemore,et al.  On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  F. Campbell,et al.  Spatial-frequency discrimination in human vision. , 1970, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[13]  W. Richards,et al.  Anomalous stereoscopic depth perception. , 1971, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[14]  M. Coltheart Visual feature-analyzers and after-effects of tilt and curvature. , 1971, Psychological review.

[15]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The orientation specificity of two visual after‐effects , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Spatial vision. , 1971, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  D. P. Andrews,et al.  Acuities for spatial arrangement in line figures: human and ideal observers compared. , 1973, Vision research.

[18]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Orientation Specificity and Spatial Selectivity in Human Vision , 1973, Perception.

[19]  J. Movshon The velocity tuning of single units in cat striate cortex. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  D Regan,et al.  The relation between discrimination and sensitivity in the perception of motion in depth. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  S P McKee,et al.  Interference with line-orientation sensitivity. , 1976, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[22]  A. Pantle,et al.  Physiological Basis of Motion Perception , 1978 .

[23]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Channels in Vision: Basic Aspects , 1978 .

[24]  J P Thomas,et al.  Bandwidths of orientation channels in human vision. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[25]  G Westheimer,et al.  The spatial sense of the eye. Proctor lecture. , 1979, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[26]  A. Dean The relationship between response amplitude and contrast for cat striate cortical neurones. , 1981, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Cross-Correlation Analysis of Interneuronal Connectivity in cat visual cortex. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Orientation Selectivity of the Human Visual System as a Function of Retinal Eccentricity and Visual Hemifield , 1981, Perception.

[29]  J. Robson,et al.  Discrimination at threshold: Labelled detectors in human vision , 1981, Vision Research.

[30]  D Regan,et al.  Spatial frequency discrimination in normal vision and in patients with multiple sclerosis. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  D. Regan Visual information channeling in normal and disordered vision. , 1982, Psychological review.

[32]  J Hirsch,et al.  Limits of spatial-frequency discrimination as evidence of neural interpolation. , 1982, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[33]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human visual orientation , 1982 .

[34]  D. Regan,et al.  Neurons in cat visual cortex tuned to the direction of motion in depth: Effect of positional disparity , 1982, Vision Research.

[35]  M. Cynader,et al.  Neurons in cat visual cortex tuned to the direction of motion in depth: effect of stimulus speed. , 1982, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[36]  J. Movshon,et al.  The statistical reliability of signals in single neurons in cat and monkey visual cortex , 1983, Vision Research.

[37]  D Regan,et al.  Independence of orientation and size in spatial discriminations. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[38]  D Regan,et al.  Spatial-frequency discrimination and detection: comparison of postadaptation thresholds. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[39]  Terry Caelli,et al.  Discrimination thresholds in the two-dimensional spatial frequency domain , 1983, Vision Research.

[40]  C. A. Burbeck,et al.  Critical problems in spatial vision. , 1984, Critical reviews in biomedical engineering.

[41]  H. Wilson,et al.  Spatial-frequency adaptation and grating discrimination: predictions of a line-element model. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[42]  D Regan,et al.  Visual responses to vorticity and the neural analysis of optic flow. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.