Zinc supplementation sustained normative neurodevelopment in a randomized, controlled trial of Peruvian infants aged 6-18 months.

A double-blind, randomized clinical trial was conducted to determine the effects of prevention of zinc deficiency on cognitive and sensorimotor development during infancy. At 6 mo of age, infants were randomly assigned to be administered a daily liquid supplement containing 10 mg/d of zinc (zinc sulfate), 10 mg/d of iron (ferrous sulfate), and 0.5 mg/d of copper (copper oxide), or an identical daily liquid supplement containing only 10 mg/d of iron and 0.5 mg/d of copper. Various controls were implemented to ensure adherence to the supplement protocol. A battery of developmental assessments was administered from 6 to 18 mo of age that included a visual habituation/recognition memory task augmented with heart rate at 6, 9, and 12 mo of age; the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd edition (BSID2) at 6, 12, and 18 mo; the A-not-B error task at 9 and 12 mo; and free-play attention tasks at 12 and 18 mo. Only infants supplemented with zinc had the normative decline in look duration from 6 to 12 mo during habituation and a normative decline in shifting between objects on free-play multiple-object attention tasks from 12 to 18 mo of age. The 2 groups did not differ on any of the psychophysiologic indices, the BSID2, or the A-not-B error task. The findings are consistent with zinc supplementation supporting a profile of normative information processing and active attentional profiles during the first 2 y of life. This trial was registered at clinicaltrials.gov as NCT00589264.

[1]  E. Courchesne,et al.  A new role for the cerebellum in cognitive operations. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[2]  J. G. Penland,et al.  Effects of repletion with zinc and other micronutrients on neuropsychologic performance and growth of Chinese children. , 1998, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[3]  N. Bayley Bayley Scales of Infant Development , 1999 .

[4]  P. Wainwright,et al.  Nutrition and the development of cognitive functions: interpretation of behavioral studies in animals and human infants. , 2006, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[5]  J. Colombo,et al.  What Habituates in Infant Visual Habituation? A Psychophysiological Analysis. , 2010, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[6]  L. Caulfield,et al.  Zinc supplementation affects the activity patterns of rural Guatemalan infants. , 1997, The Journal of nutrition.

[7]  L. Caulfield,et al.  Mineral status of non-anemic Peruvian infants taking an iron and copper syrup with or without zinc from 6 to 18 months of age: a randomized controlled trial. , 2013, Nutrition.

[8]  J. Colombo,et al.  A parametric study of the infant control procedure , 1985 .

[9]  H. Stenlund,et al.  A community-based randomized controlled trial of iron and zinc supplementation in Indonesian infants: effects on growth and development. , 2004, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[10]  J. Colombo Recent advances in infant cognition: Implications for long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation studies , 2001, Lipids.

[11]  R. Gibson,et al.  A growth-limiting, mild zinc-deficiency syndrome in some southern Ontario boys with low height percentiles. , 1989, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[12]  J. Katz,et al.  Inconsistent Effects of Iron-Folic Acid and/or Zinc Supplementation on the Cognitive Development of Infants , 2011, Journal of health, population, and nutrition.

[13]  L. Caulfield,et al.  Adding zinc to prenatal iron and folate supplements improves maternal and neonatal zinc status in a Peruvian population. , 1999, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[14]  L. Allen,et al.  Complementary feeding of young children in developing countries: a review of current scientific knowledge. , 1998 .

[15]  R. Black,et al.  Effect of zinc supplementation on observed activity in low socioeconomic Indian preschool children. , 1996, Pediatrics.

[16]  J. Colombo,et al.  Individual differences in infant visual attention: are short lookers faster processors or feature processors? , 1991, Child development.

[17]  J. Friel,et al.  Zinc supplementation in very-low-birth-weight infants. , 1993, Journal of pediatric gastroenterology and nutrition.

[18]  Terrill F. Saxon,et al.  Individual and developmental differences in disengagement of fixation in early infancy. , 1999, Child development.

[19]  J. G. Penland,et al.  A preliminary report: effects of zinc and micronutrient repletion on growth and neuropsychological function of urban Chinese children. , 1997, Journal of the American College of Nutrition.

[20]  D. Amso,et al.  Contributions of Neuroscience to Our Understanding of Cognitive Development , 2008, Current directions in psychological science.

[21]  N. Krebs Dietary zinc and iron sources, physical growth and cognitive development of breastfed infants. , 2000, The Journal of nutrition.

[22]  S. Carlson,et al.  Is the Measure the Message: The BSID and Nutritional Interventions , 2012, Pediatrics.

[23]  John Colombo,et al.  Infant Pathways to Language : Methods, Models, and Research Directions , 2008 .

[24]  T. Smart,et al.  Modulation of inhibitory and excitatory amino acid receptor ion channels by zinc , 1994, Progress in Neurobiology.

[25]  J. Richards The development of sustained visual attention in infants from 14 to 26 weeks of age. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[26]  J. Richards,et al.  Development and stability in visual sustained attention in 14, 20, and 26 week old infants. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[27]  R. Black,et al.  Cognitive and motor development among small-for-gestational-age infants: impact of zinc supplementation, birth weight, and caregiving practices. , 2004, Pediatrics.

[28]  John Colombo,et al.  Visual processing and infant ocular Latencies in the overlap paradigm. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[29]  E. Hertrampf,et al.  Effect of zinc supplementation on development and growth of Chilean infants. , 2001, The Journal of pediatrics.

[30]  S. Taneja,et al.  Impact of zinc supplementation on mental and psychomotor scores of children aged 12 to 18 months: a randomized, double-blind trial. , 2005, The Journal of pediatrics.

[31]  Ronald A. Cohen,et al.  Spatial Determinants of Attention , 1993 .

[32]  C. Frederickson,et al.  A zinc‐containing fiber system of thalamic origin , 1994, NeuroReport.

[33]  J. Colombo,et al.  Infant visual attention in the paired-comparison paradigm: test-retest and attention-performance relations. , 1988, Child development.

[34]  D. J. Shaddy,et al.  The Developmental Course of Habituation in Infancy and Preschool Outcome , 2004 .

[35]  John Colombo,et al.  individual Differences in infancy : Reliability, Stability, and Prediction , 1990 .

[36]  Carol L Cheatham,et al.  Long-term effects of LCPUFA supplementation on childhood cognitive outcomes. , 2013, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[37]  Lindsay H Allen,et al.  Combined iron and folic acid supplementation with or without zinc reduces time to walking unassisted among Zanzibari infants 5- to 11-mo old. , 2006, The Journal of nutrition.

[38]  B. Lozoff,et al.  Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. , 2003, Pediatrics.

[39]  H. Sandstead Zinc deficiency. A public health problem? , 1991, American journal of diseases of children.

[40]  P. Trumbo,et al.  Dietary reference intakes: vitamin A, vitamin K, arsenic, boron, chromium, copper, iodine, iron, manganese, molybdenum, nickel, silicon, vanadium, and zinc. , 1998, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[41]  R. Cohen The Neuropsychology of Attention , 1993, Critical Issues in Neuropsychology.

[42]  H. Pan,et al.  WHO child growth standards: length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age , 2006 .

[43]  K. Pribram,et al.  Arousal, activation, and effort in the control of attention. , 1975, Psychological review.

[44]  Lisa M. Oakes,et al.  The Development of Attention and Its Relations to Language in Infancy and Toddlerhood , 2008 .

[45]  M. Black Zinc deficiency and child development. , 1998, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[46]  its Panel on Folate,et al.  FOOD AND NUTRITION BOARD , 1998 .

[47]  J. Colombo,et al.  The stability of visual habituation during the first year of life. , 1987, Child development.

[48]  Susan E Carlson,et al.  Maternal DHA and the development of attention in infancy and toddlerhood. , 2004, Child development.

[49]  E. N. Sokolov,et al.  Perception and the Conditioned Reflex , 1965 .

[50]  A. Ashworth,et al.  Zinc supplementation, mental development and behaviour in low birth weight term infants in northeast Brazil , 1998, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[51]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[52]  M. Golub,et al.  Developmental zinc deficiency and behavior. , 1995, The Journal of nutrition.

[53]  J. Colombo,et al.  Heart rate-defined phases of attention, look duration, and infant performance in the paired-comparison paradigm. , 2001, Child development.

[54]  F. Khatun,et al.  Randomized controlled trial of the effect of zinc supplementation on the mental development of Bangladeshi infants. , 2001, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[55]  R. Thompson,et al.  Importance of zinc in the central nervous system: the zinc-containing neuron. , 2000, The Journal of nutrition.

[56]  J. Colombo Infant Cognition: Predicting Later Intellectual Functioning , 1993 .

[57]  L. Caulfield,et al.  Dietary pattern and usual nutrient intakes of Peruvian women during pregnancy , 2003, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[58]  A. Diamond,et al.  Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. , 1991, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[59]  Adele Diamond,et al.  Understanding the A‐not‐B Error: Working memory vs. reinforced response, or active trace vs. latent trace , 1998 .