An evaluation of evidence supporting fact-retrieval models

Abstract This literature review compares fact-retrieval models with a schema-based view, which proposes that relational knowledge is a critical component in learning and representing basic number facts such as 8 × 3 = 24. It then critically analyzes evidence adduced to support fact-retrieval models and points out plausible alternative explanations for these results. For example, data collected on mental-arithmetic errors frequently confound retrieved responses with nonretrieved responses. Thus, factor-related errors (e.g., 8 × 3 = 21) and close-miss errors (e.g., 8 × 3 = 23) may, at least in part, be the by-product of fast back-up strategies and may not accurately reflect the mental representation of number facts. Moreover, contrary to Siegler's (1988) fact-retrieval model and consistent with the schema-based view, such errors may increase on unpracticed combinations. The article concludes with recommendations for more clearly determining how the basic number facts are learned and represented.

[1]  R. Siegler The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children's addition. , 1987 .

[2]  A. Baroody Children's mathematical thinking , 1987 .

[3]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Retrieval inhibition and interference in cognitive arithmetic , 1990 .

[4]  O. Svenson Memory retrieval of answers of simple additions as reflected in response latencies , 1985 .

[5]  Susan R. Goldman,et al.  Extended Practice of Basic Addition Facts: Strategy Changes in Learning-Disabled Students , 1988 .

[6]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Children’s Knowledge of Simple Arithmetic: A Developmental Model and Simulation , 1987 .

[7]  M. Ashcraft,et al.  The development of children's mental multiplication skills , 1991 .

[8]  M. Ashcraft Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.

[9]  N J Cohen,et al.  Cognitive representations and processes in arithmetic: inferences from the performance of brain-damaged subjects. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  H. Lee Swanson,et al.  Acquisition of Mental Multiplication Skill: Evidence for the Transition Between Counting and Retrieval Strategies , 1988 .

[11]  P. Nesher Learning Mathematics: A Cognitive Perspective. , 1986 .

[12]  R. Siegler,et al.  Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skill. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  David R. Paredes,et al.  Starting to add worse: Effects of learning to multiply on children's addition , 1990, Cognition.

[14]  R. E. Eicholz Addison-Wesley mathematics , 1985 .

[15]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Mental multiplication skill: Structure, process, and acquisition. , 1985 .

[16]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  Children's Use of Mathematical Structure. , 1983 .

[17]  A. Baroody Children's Mathematical Thinking: A Developmental Framework for Preschool, Primary, and Special Education Teachers , 1987 .

[18]  Clyde A. Dilley,et al.  Heath elementary mathematics , 1975 .

[19]  Charles E. Browne The Psychology of the Simple Arithmetical Processes: A Study of Certain Habits of Attention and Association , 1906 .

[20]  J. Pellegrino,et al.  Mental chronometry and individual differences in cognitive processes: Common pitfalls and their solutions , 1989 .

[21]  D. Kaye The development of mathematical cognition , 1986 .

[22]  The development of kindergartners' mental-addition strategies , 1992 .

[23]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric approach☆ , 1982 .

[24]  G D Logan,et al.  On the autonomy of mental processes: a case study of arithmetic. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[25]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[26]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  The development of procedural knowledge: An alternative explanation for chronometric trends of mental arithmetic , 1983 .

[27]  Guy J. Groen,et al.  Temporal aspects of simple addition and comparison , 1971 .

[28]  A. Baroody Mental-addition development of children classified as mentally handicapped , 1988 .

[29]  Eric A. Jenkins,et al.  How Children Discover New Strategies , 1989 .

[30]  M. Posner Chronometric explorations of mind , 1978 .

[31]  John R. Anderson Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .

[32]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  A reexamination of mental arithmetic models and data: A reply to Ashcraft , 1984 .

[33]  Timothy A. Post,et al.  Emergence of Information-Retrieval Strategies in Numerical Cognition: A Developmental Study , 1986 .

[34]  Michael McCloskey,et al.  Models of arithmetic fact retrieval: an evaluation in light of findings from normal and brain-damaged subjects. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  P Cormier,et al.  A componential model for mental addition. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  Arthur J. Baroody Kindergartners' Mental Addition with Single-Digit Combinations , 1989 .

[37]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  Mastery of Basic Number Combinations : Internalization of Relationships or Facts ? , 2016 .

[38]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for retrieval and decision processes in mental addition. , 1978 .

[39]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Textbook Presentations of the Basic Addition Facts , 1986 .

[40]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Time course of error priming in number-fact retrieval: evidence for excitatory and inhibitory mechanisms , 1989 .

[41]  John R. Anderson The Architecture of Cognition , 1983 .

[42]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  On the relation between production and verification tasks in the psychology of simple arithmetic , 1990 .

[43]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[44]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .

[45]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Network interference and mental multiplication. , 1987 .

[46]  F. B. Knight,et al.  The learning of the 100 addition combinations and the 100 subtraction combinations , 1928 .

[47]  G. Groen,et al.  A chronometric analysis of simple addition. , 1972 .

[48]  Max E. Jerman Some Strategies for Solving Simple Multiplication Combinations. , 1970 .

[49]  A. Baroody Early Mental Multiplication Performance and the Role of Relational Knowledge in Mastering Combinations Involving "Two.". , 1993 .

[50]  Janet A. Schmidt,et al.  Associative confusions in mental arithmetic , 1974 .

[51]  J. Hiebert Conceptual and procedural knowledge : the case of mathematics , 1987 .

[52]  J. I. Campbell Production, verification, and priming of multiplication facts , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[53]  M. Perlmutter,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: comparison of operations. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[54]  M. Ashcraft Procedural knowledge versus fact retrieval in mental arithmetic: A reply to Baroody☆ , 1983 .