Efficacy of navigation may be influenced by retrosplenial cortex-mediated learning of landmark stability

ABSTRACT Human beings differ considerably in their ability to orient and navigate within the environment, but it has been difficult to determine specific causes of these individual differences. Permanent, stable landmarks are thought to be crucial for building a mental representation of an environment. Poor, compared to good, navigators have been shown to have difficulty identifying permanent landmarks, with a concomitant reduction in functional MRI (fMRI) activity in the retrosplenial cortex. However, a clear association between navigation ability and the learning of permanent landmarks has not been established. Here we tested for such a link. We had participants learn a virtual reality environment by repeatedly moving through it during fMRI scanning. The environment contained landmarks of which participants had no prior experience, some of which remained fixed in their locations while others changed position each time they were seen. After the fMRI learning phase, we divided participants into good and poor navigators based on their ability to find their way in the environment. The groups were closely matched on a range of cognitive and structural brain measures. Examination of the learning phase during scanning revealed that, while good and poor navigators learned to recognise the environment's landmarks at a similar rate, poor navigators were impaired at registering whether landmarks were stable or transient, and this was associated with reduced engagement of the retrosplenial cortex. Moreover, a mediation analysis showed that there was a significant effect of landmark permanence learning on navigation performance mediated through retrosplenial cortex activity. We conclude that a diminished ability to process landmark permanence may be a contributory factor to sub‐optimal navigation, and could be related to the level of retrosplenial cortex engagement. HIGHLIGHTSPeople learned the layout of a virtual environment during fMRI scanning.Wayfinding was tested after the learning phase and good/poor navigators identified.Poor navigators were impaired at registering landmark permanence during learning.This was accompanied by reduced retrosplenial cortex activity.

[1]  M. Petrides,et al.  Retrosplenial and hippocampal brain regions in human navigation: complementary functional contributions to the formation and use of cognitive maps , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[2]  Peter Zeidman,et al.  A central role for the retrosplenial cortex in de novo environmental learning , 2015, eLife.

[3]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Dissociable networks involved in spatial and temporal order source retrieval , 2011, NeuroImage.

[4]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: A whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T , 2006, NeuroImage.

[5]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Multiple reference frames used by the human brain for spatial perception and memory , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Neural Encoding of Objects Relevant for Navigation and Resting State Correlations with Navigational Ability , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  A. Oliva,et al.  A Real-World Size Organization of Object Responses in Occipitotemporal Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[8]  C. Donald Heth,et al.  Self-ratings of sense of direction and route reversal performance , 2002 .

[9]  H. Duvernoy,et al.  The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy with MRI, and Blood Supply , 1999 .

[10]  Elizabeth R. Chrastil,et al.  Individual Differences in Human Path Integration Abilities Correlate with Gray Matter Volume in Retrosplenial Cortex, Hippocampus, and Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2017, eNeuro.

[11]  A. Lew Looking beyond the boundaries: time to put landmarks back on the cognitive map? , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  Nigel Foreman,et al.  Exploratory activity and response to a spatial change in rats with hippocampal or posterior parietal cortical lesions , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  Anna S. Mitchell,et al.  Retrosplenial Cortical Contributions to Anterograde and Retrograde Memory in the Monkey , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Assessing the mechanism of response in the retrosplenial cortex of good and poor navigators☆ , 2013, Cortex.

[15]  Nicole Etchamendy,et al.  Spontaneous navigational strategies and performance in the virtual town , 2007, Hippocampus.

[16]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Development of a self-report measure of environmental spatial ability. , 2002 .

[17]  Simon M Stringer,et al.  Optimal cue combination and landmark‐stability learning in the head direction system , 2016, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  Sharon L. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Learning Places from Views: Variation in Scene Processing as a Function of Experience and Navigational Ability , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Hodges,et al.  Lost in spatial translation – A novel tool to objectively assess spatial disorientation in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia , 2015, Cortex.

[20]  Kate Jeffery,et al.  An independent, landmark-dominated head direction signal in dysgranular retrosplenial cortex , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Anchoring the neural compass: Coding of local spatial reference frames in human medial parietal lobe , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  E. Maguire,et al.  What does the retrosplenial cortex do? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  Eleanor A Maguire,et al.  A New Role for the Parahippocampal Cortex in Representing Space , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  C. Büchel,et al.  Dissociable Retrosplenial and Hippocampal Contributions to Successful Formation of Survey Representations , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Retrosplenial Cortex Codes for Permanent Landmarks , 2012, PloS one.

[26]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Neural correlates of real-world route learning , 2010, NeuroImage.

[27]  Jeffrey S. Taube,et al.  Origins of landmark encoding in the brain , 2011, Trends in Neurosciences.

[28]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Gray Matter Differences Correlate with Spontaneous Strategies in a Human Virtual Navigation Task , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[30]  John P. Aggleton,et al.  Understanding retrosplenial amnesia: Insights from animal studies , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  D. Hassabis,et al.  A Goal Direction Signal in the Human Entorhinal/Subicular Region , 2015, Current Biology.

[32]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Multiple object properties drive scene-selective regions. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  E. Maguire,et al.  Acquiring “the Knowledge” of London's Layout Drives Structural Brain Changes , 2011, Current Biology.

[35]  J. Hodges,et al.  Focal posterior cingulate atrophy in incipient Alzheimer's disease , 2010, Neurobiology of Aging.

[36]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Memory consolidation of landmarks in good navigators , 2008, Hippocampus.

[38]  R Turner,et al.  Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T , 2004, NeuroImage.

[39]  J. Hodges,et al.  Retrosplenial Cortex (BA 29) Volumes in Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia and Alzheimer’s Disease , 2013, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders.

[40]  E. J. Green,et al.  Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Dissociable neural circuits for encoding and retrieval of object locations during active navigation in humans , 2010, NeuroImage.

[42]  Kevin Lynch,et al.  The Image of the City , 1960 .

[43]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Hippocampal size predicts rapid learning of a cognitive map in humans , 2013, Hippocampus.

[44]  T. Ohnishi,et al.  Navigation ability dependent neural activation in the human brain: An fMRI study , 2006, Neuroscience Research.

[45]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Navigational skills correlate with hippocampal fractional anisotropy in humans , 2008, Hippocampus.

[46]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Voxel-Based Morphometry—The Methods , 2000, NeuroImage.

[47]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[48]  A. Anastasi Individual differences. , 2020, Annual review of psychology.

[49]  E. Maguire The retrosplenial contribution to human navigation: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. , 2001, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[50]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Selective neural representation of objects relevant for navigation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[51]  Howard Eichenbaum,et al.  A cognitive map for object memory in the hippocampus. , 2009, Learning & memory.

[52]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence , 2014 .

[53]  E. Luders,et al.  Voxel-Based Morphometry , 2015 .

[54]  J R Hodges,et al.  Retrosplenial cortex (BA 29/30) hypometabolism in mild cognitive impairment (prodromal Alzheimer's disease) , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[55]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[56]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  The Human Retrosplenial Cortex and Thalamus Code Head Direction in a Global Reference Frame , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  What determines our navigational abilities? , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Reference Frames for Spatial Cognition: Different Brain Areas are Involved in Viewer-, Object-, and Landmark-Centered Judgments About Object Location , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Outside Looking In: Landmark Generalization in the Human Navigational System , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  P. E. Sharp,et al.  Head direction, place, and movement correlates for cells in the rat retrosplenial cortex. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[61]  Gaspare Galati,et al.  Functional connectivity between posterior hippocampus and retrosplenial complex predicts individual differences in navigational ability , 2016, Hippocampus.

[62]  Jiye G. Kim,et al.  A Neural Basis for Developmental Topographic Disorientation , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  Caswell Barry,et al.  Neural systems supporting navigation , 2015, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.