Is perception isomorphic with neural activity?

The theory of visual stability proposed by Bridgeman et al. requires die existence of retinotopic maps in which neurons represent the spatiotopic position of objects through their activity. The authors reason that if the code for position is fully specified by the neural activity, it becomes independent of the position of neurons within the map (sect. 4.1, para. 3). These two aspects of Bridgeman et al.'s theory, namely, the retinotopy of the maps and the encoding of position through the activity regardless of retinotopic position, appear difficult to reconcile. Neurons within retinotopic maps in early visual cortex are known to have small receptive fields. Accordingly, a single cell cannot encode all possible retinal positions through its firing rate. If the head and trunk are fixed, the position of a11 object in space is the vector sum of its retinal position and eye position. Ifacell cannot encode all possible retinalpositions, it is difficult to imagine how it could do so for spatiotopic positions. This in turn entails that a neuronal population at a given location on the map cannot possibly encode an arbitrary head-centered position through its activity. The problem stems from the fact that in computing spatiotopic position in retinotopic maps, there is an intimate relation between what Bridgeman et al. call the inode ofrepresenting and the position of representing (sect. 4, para. 1). One relevant variable, retinal position, is itself represented by the map. As emphasized in the target article, this is not a problem for other visual attributes such as color or motion (sect. 4, para. 5-6). Extremely large receptive fields would certainly solve the problem (Zipser & Andersen 1988), but neurons in V1 or V2 do not have such large receptive fields. Nevertheless, it is still possible that the early visual cortical areas may support, partly or fully, a representation of the visual field that is suitable for determining visual stability during eye movements. We have presented a model for one way this could be done in a recent paper (Pouget e t al. 1993), inspired by the reports from several laboratories that eye position modulates visually driven activity in the lateral geniculate nucleus (Lal & Friedlander 1989; Lehmkuhle & Bar,o 1991) and area V1 and V3a of visual cortex (Galleti & Battaglini 1989; Trotter e t al. 1992; Weyand & Malpeli 1993). We have shown that such neurons could support a distributed representation of objects, including their head-centered spatial locations; the location is encoded by the modulationofthe visual responses ofneurons by eye position. We called this representation a retinospatiotopic map. The code for spatiotopic position in a retinospatiotopic map depends on both the pattern of activity and the s or, equivalently, if and only if positions in space are in a one-toone relationship with patterns of neuronal activity. Whether the patterns move to different locations in the brain as a result of eye movements is irrelevant for them, as long as the pattern stays the same. A retinospatiotopic map fails because it docs not encode head-centered position solely through an activity pattern but through a coinhination of activity and topography. The isomorphism criterion may be too strict, since even brain regions such as parietal area 7a and the lateral intraparietal area (LIP), which are believed to represent the position of objects in spatiotopic coordinates, would not qualify according to this strict requirement. Nonetheless, area 7a appears to contain a distributed representation of objects in body-centered coordinates (Andersen & Zipser 1988; Goodman & Andersen 1990; Zipser & Andersen 1988). It is possible that there is no invariant representation of spatial location anywhere in the brain. Would this make any computational problem that the brain must solve insoluble as a consequence? On the contrary, any invariant function, such as grasp of an object in space, could be computed from a distributed representation. Objects are grasped, brain activity comes and goes, and no invariant representation is to be found, anywhere, except perhaps at the level of the motoneurons. The strong isomorphism hypothesis is a conceptually simple one that would be convenient if true, but by no means necessary. We must begin to imagine how it could be that more than one pattern of activity can represent the same percept. Models provide a way to embody this otherwise difficult to accept possibility. This leaves open the question of what it is that constitutes the feeling that the world is stable, which may have quite a different answer from the question of how the stable world is represented. Note that stability has a much lower dimensionality than the world itself in the simplest case we need only a single scalar neuron that monitors stability. Such a stability monitor would need to receive converging inputs from many part of the brain and would act as a comparator for successive brain states. The brainstem would be a more likely place to find such neurons than the cortex. Such a system could be useful for balance and might even contribute to the control of posture. It could not, however, be used to coordinate transformations between sensory modalities, nor would it help in guiding actions. There is no reason different representations of the stable world might not be found in different parts of the brain for different purposes.

[1]  Charles Bell,et al.  On the Motions of the Eye, in Illustration of the Uses of the Muscles and Nerves of the Orbit , 1815 .

[2]  C. Sherrington Further note on the sensory nerves of the eye-muscles , 1899, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.

[3]  E. Mach Die analyse der empfindungen und das verhältniss des physischen zum psychischen , 1902 .

[4]  C. Sherrington OBSERVATIONS ON THE SENSUAL RÔLE OF THE PROPRIOCEPTIVE NERVE-SUPPLY OF THE EXTRINSIC OCULAR MUSCLES , 1918 .

[5]  J. Uexküll Umwelt und Innenwelt der Tiere , 1921 .

[6]  W. Metzger Gesetze des Sehens , 1937 .

[7]  G. L. Walls,et al.  The Vertebrate Eye and Its Adaptive Radiation. , 2013 .

[8]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[9]  J. Gibson The perception of the visual world , 1951 .

[10]  E. Ludvigh Control of ocular movements and visual interpretation of environment. , 1952, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[11]  E. Ludvigh Possible role of proprioception in the extraocular muscles. , 1952, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[12]  E. Heinemann,et al.  Über Aufbau und Wandlungen der Wahrnehmungswelt , 1953 .

[13]  E. Holst Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs , 1954 .

[14]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[15]  R. Selle Spot Chromatography Identification of Citrus Rootstocks , 1958, Nature.

[16]  D. Mackay Perceptual Stability of a Stroboscopically Lit Visual Field containing Self-Luminous Objects , 1958, Nature.

[17]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  The analysis of behavior in terms of control systems , 1958 .

[18]  J. M. Notterman,et al.  Demonstration of the Influence of Stimulus and Response Categories upon Difference Limens , 1960, Science.

[19]  I. Rock,et al.  Stroboscopic movement based on change of phenomenal rather than retinal location. , 1962, The American journal of psychology.

[20]  J. M. Notterman,et al.  Visual Velocity Discrimination: Effects of Spatial and Temporal Cues , 1962, Science.

[21]  W. S. McCulloch,et al.  Aspects of the Theory of Artificial Intelligence , 1962, Springer US.

[22]  L. Kaufman,et al.  The Moon Illusion, II , 1962, Science.

[23]  P. A. Kolers THE ILLUSION OF MOVEMENT. , 1964, Scientific American.

[24]  K. Heller Die Analyse der Empfindungen , 1964 .

[25]  T. Parks POST-RETINAL VISUAL STORAGE. , 1965, The American journal of psychology.

[26]  H. Wallach,et al.  The effect of abnormal displacement of the retinal image during eye movements , 1966 .

[27]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[28]  C. Trevarthen,et al.  Two mechanisms of vision in primates , 1968, Psychologische Forschung.

[29]  N. Bischof,et al.  [Investigations and considerations of directional perception during voluntary saccadic eye movements]. , 1968, Psychologische Forschung.

[30]  R. Held Dissociation of visual functions by deprivation and rearrangement , 1968 .

[31]  N. Bischof,et al.  Untersuchungen und Überlegungen zur Richtungswahrnehmung bei willkürlichen sakkadischen Augenbewegungen , 1968 .

[32]  F. C. Volkmann,et al.  Time course of visual inhibition during voluntary saccades. , 1968, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[33]  C. H. Graham,et al.  Effect of wavelength on foveal grating acuity. , 1968, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[34]  R. Wurtz Response of striate cortex neurons to stimuli during rapid eye movements in the monkey. , 1969, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  A. Mack An investigation of the relationship between eye and retinal image movement in the perception of movement , 1970 .

[36]  R. Haber,et al.  Contemporary theory and research in visual perception , 1970 .

[37]  P. Dodwell Perceptual processing : stimulus equivalence and pattern recognition , 1971 .

[38]  R. Held,et al.  Moving Visual Scenes Influence the Apparent Direction of Gravity , 1972, Science.

[39]  H. Wallach,et al.  Adaptation in distance perception based on oculomotor cues , 1972 .

[40]  S. Coren,et al.  Attention : contemporary theory and analysis , 1972 .

[41]  A. Stoper Apparent motion of stimuli presented stroboscopically during pursuit movement of the eye , 1973 .

[42]  A. P. Petrov,et al.  Torsional eye movements and constancy of the visual field. , 1973, Vision research.

[43]  W. Epstein,et al.  The Process of ‘Taking-into-Account’ in Visual Perception , 1973, Perception.

[44]  H. Ridley Eye and Brain , 1973 .

[45]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[46]  A. O. Dick,et al.  Effect of eye movements on backward masking and perceived location , 1973 .

[47]  E. Keller Participation of medial pontine reticular formation in eye movement generation in monkey. , 1974, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  L. Riggs,et al.  Suppression of visual phosphenes during saccadic eye movements. , 1974, Vision research.

[49]  J Paillard,et al.  A proprioceptive contribution to the spatial encoding of position cues for ballistic movements. , 1974, Brain research.

[50]  L. Stark,et al.  Time optimal behavior of human saccadic eye movement , 1975 .

[51]  B. A. Brooks,et al.  Influence of stimulus parameters on visual sensitivity during saccadic eye movement , 1975, Vision Research.

[52]  M. Sanders Handbook of Sensory Physiology , 1975 .

[53]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[54]  A. Kornheiser Adaptation to laterally displaced vision: a review. , 1976, Psychological bulletin.

[55]  R. C. Emerson,et al.  Paralysis of the awake human: Visual perceptions , 1976, Vision Research.

[56]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades , 1976, Vision Research.

[57]  F. Leukel Introduction to physiological psychology , 1976 .

[58]  A. R. Lurii︠a︡,et al.  Basic Problems of Neurolinguistics , 1976 .

[59]  J. Senders,et al.  Eye Movements and Psychological Processes , 1976 .

[60]  L. Stark,et al.  Normal saccadic structure of voluntary nystagmus. , 1977, Archives of ophthalmology.

[61]  A. Trehub,et al.  Neuronal models for cognitive processes: networks for learning, perception and imagination. , 1977, Journal of theoretical biology.

[62]  A. A. Skavenski,et al.  Accuracy of eye position information for motor control , 1977, Vision Research.

[63]  B. Bridgeman Reply to Brooks and Fuchs: Exogenous and endogenous contributions to saccadic suppression , 1977, Vision Research.

[64]  M. P. Friedman,et al.  HANDBOOK OF PERCEPTION , 1977 .

[65]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Contrasting orientations to the theory of visual information processing. , 1977 .

[66]  W. Epstein Stability and constancy in visual perception : mechanisms and processes , 1977 .

[67]  C Bonnet,et al.  Visual Motion Detection Models: Features and Frequency Filters , 1977, Perception.

[68]  C. L. Morgan Constancy of egocentric visual direction , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[69]  S. C. Wong,et al.  Subjective motion and acceleration induced by the movement of the observer’s entire visual field , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[70]  S. Mateeff Saccadic eye movements and localization of visual stimuli , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[71]  F. Campbell,et al.  Saccadic omission: Why we do not see a grey-out during a saccadic eye movement , 1978, Vision Research.

[72]  W. Gogel The adjacency principle in visual percerption. , 1978, Scientific American.

[73]  L. Stark,et al.  Omnidirectional increase in threshold for image shifts during saccadic eye movements , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[74]  E. Rosch,et al.  Cognition and Categorization , 1980 .

[75]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[76]  C. Hofsten Recalibration of the Convergence System , 1979 .

[77]  W. Uttal Do central nonlinearities exist? , 1979, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[78]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979 .

[79]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Iconic memory and visible persistence , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[80]  P. Thompson,et al.  Margaret Thatcher: A New Illusion , 1980, Perception.

[81]  D. A. Owens,et al.  Accommodation, convergence, and distance perception in low illumination. , 1980, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[82]  L. Stark,et al.  Voluntary nystagmus, saccadic suppression, and stabilization of the visual world , 1980, Vision Research.

[83]  W. Wolf,et al.  Interaction of pre- and postsaccadic patterns having the same coordinates in space , 1980, Vision Research.

[84]  J. Moran,et al.  Sensation and perception , 1980 .

[85]  P Conti,et al.  Role of Structured Visual Field and Visual Reafference in Accuracy of Pointing Movements , 1980, Perceptual and motor skills.

[86]  B. Bridgeman Cognitive factors in subjective stabilization of the visual world. , 1981, Acta psychologica.

[87]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[88]  A. Bahill,et al.  Variability and development of a normative data base for saccadic eye movements. , 1981, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[89]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Integrating visual information from successive fixations. , 1982, Science.

[90]  David Marr,et al.  VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .

[91]  D. F. Fisher,et al.  Eye movements : cognition and visual perception , 1982 .

[92]  B. Julesz,et al.  The existence and role of retinotopic and spatiotopic forms of visual persistence. , 1982, Acta psychologica.

[93]  E. Reed An outline of a theory of action systems. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[94]  J. Stevens,et al.  Oculoparalytic illusion: visual-field dependent spatial mislocalizations by humans partially paralyzed with curare. , 1982, Science.

[95]  H. Maturana What is it to see? , 1983, Archivos de biologia y medicina experimentales.

[96]  R. Haber The impending demise of the icon: A critique of the concept of iconic storage in visual information processing , 1983, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[97]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to integrate visual information from successive fixations , 1983 .

[98]  H. Maturana What is to see ?. Comparative neurobiology of vision in vertebrates an international symposium. Punta de tralca, chile. 25-27 november, 1982 , 1983 .

[99]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[100]  L. Stark,et al.  Role of corollary discharge in space constancy , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[101]  K. Rayner,et al.  Is visual information integrated across saccades? , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[102]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Evidence against visual integration across saccadic eye movements , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[103]  R. B. Freeman,et al.  Cognition and Motor Processes , 1983 .

[104]  Jonathan Stone,et al.  Parallel Processing in the Visual System , 1983, Perspectives in Vision Research.

[105]  W. Shebilske,et al.  Efferent factors in natural event perception can be rationalized and verified: a reply to Turvey and Solomon. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[106]  J. T. Enright Changes in vergence mediated by saccades. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[107]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Human smooth and saccadic eye movements during voluntary pursuit of different target motions on different backgrounds. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[108]  R. Shaw,et al.  Persistence and Change : Proceedings of the First International Conference on Event Perception , 1985 .

[109]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[110]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Eye movements and stereopsis during dichoptic viewing of moving random-dot stereograms , 1985, Vision Research.

[111]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[112]  D. Ingle,et al.  Brain mechanisms and spatial vision , 1985 .

[113]  S. Sherman Functional organization of the W-, X-, and Y- cell pathways in the cat: A review and hypothesis , 1985 .

[114]  H. Leibowitz,et al.  A revised analysis of the role of efference in motion perception. , 1985, Perception.

[115]  Ronald M. Hansen,et al.  Accuracy of spatial localizations near the time of saccadic eye movements , 1985, Vision Research.

[116]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Biological image motion processing: A review , 1985, Vision Research.

[117]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Motion perception during dichoptic viewing of moving random-dot stereograms , 1985, Vision Research.

[118]  J. Stern Theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research A. G. Gale and F. Johnson, (Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., Amsterdam, 1984) pp. xiii + 565, Dfl. 185 , 1985, Biological Psychology.

[119]  J. Feldman Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[120]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[121]  O. Grüsser,et al.  Interaction of efferent and afferent signals in visual perception. A history of ideas and experimental paradigms. , 1986, Acta psychologica.

[122]  B Bridgeman,et al.  Multiple sources of outflow in processing spatial information. , 1986, Acta psychologica.

[123]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Is stimulus persistence affected by eye movements? , 1986, Psychological research.

[124]  O. Grüsser,et al.  Afterimage movement during saccades in the dark , 1987, Vision Research.

[125]  M. Steinbach Proprioceptive knowledge of eye position , 1987, Vision Research.

[126]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Cognitive Processes and Spatial Orientation in Animal and Man , 1987 .

[127]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Human gaze stability in the horizontal, vertical and torsional direction during voluntary head movements, evaluated with a three-dimensional scleral induction coil technique , 1987, Vision Research.

[128]  DH Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, and stereopsis in primate area 18 , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[129]  L. Vaina Matters of Intelligence , 1987 .

[130]  J. Kaas The organization of neocortex in mammals: implications for theories of brain function. , 1987, Annual review of psychology.

[131]  D. Bouwhuis Sensorimotor interactions in space perception and action , 1987 .

[132]  J. T. Enright The cyclopean eye and its implications: Vergence state and visual direction , 1988, Vision Research.

[133]  D. O. Hebb,et al.  The organization of behavior , 1988 .

[134]  R. Andersen,et al.  The role of the posterior parietal cortex in coordinate transformations for visual-motor integration. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[135]  Han Collewijn,et al.  PART II. THREE‐DIMENSIONAL CODING IN THE OCULOMOTOR AND VISUAL SYSTEMS The Behavior of Human Gaze in Three Dimensions a , 1988 .

[136]  Uwe Windhorst,et al.  How Brain-Like Is the Spinal Cord: Interacting Cell Assemblies in the Nervous System , 1988 .

[137]  E. Reed James J. Gibson and the psychology of perception , 1988 .

[138]  Professor Dr. Uwe Windhorst How Brain-like is the Spinal Cord? , 1988, Studies of Brain Function.

[139]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[140]  Z W Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.

[141]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[142]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Visual masking and visual integration across saccadic eye movements. , 1988 .

[143]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons , 1988, Nature.

[144]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Differences in accuracy of human saccades between stationary and jumping targets , 1989, Vision Research.

[145]  S. Mateeff,et al.  The Role of the Adjacency between Background Cues and Objects in Visual Localization during Ocular Pursuit , 1989, Perception.

[146]  C. Galletti,et al.  Gaze-dependent visual neurons in area V3A of monkey prestriate cortex , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[147]  H. Heuer,et al.  Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .

[148]  R. Lal,et al.  Gating of retinal transmission by afferent eye position and movement signals. , 1989, Science.

[149]  M. Woollacott,et al.  Development of Posture and Gait: Across the Life Span , 1989 .

[150]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Influence of mechanical disturbance on oculomotor behavior , 1989, Vision Research.

[151]  L. Spillmann,et al.  Visual Perception: The Neurophysiological Foundations , 1989 .

[152]  J. Ewert,et al.  Visuomotor Coordination: Amphibians, Comparisons, Models, and Robots , 1989 .

[153]  G. Ettlinger,et al.  The neural structures involved in cross-modal recognition and tactile discrimination performance: An investigation using 2-DG , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[154]  G. W. Strong,et al.  A solution to the tag-assignment problem for neural networks , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[155]  Stephen R. Ellis,et al.  Spatial Displays and Spatial Instruments , 1989 .

[156]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion , 1989, Vision Research.

[157]  J. A. Graziano,et al.  Effect of context and efference copy on visual straight ahead , 1989, Vision Research.

[158]  R. Melzack Pain and the neuromatrix in the brain. , 2001, Journal of dental education.

[159]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of processing: A framework for memory research , 1972 .

[160]  G. Ettlinger “Object Vision” and “Spatial Vision”: The Neuropsychological Evidence for the Distinction , 1990, Cortex.

[161]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Saccadic Suppression of Displacement is Strongest in Central Vision , 1990, Perception.

[162]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction , 1990, Nature.

[163]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Colliding saccades may reveal the secret of their marching orders , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[164]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Algorithm programmed by a neural network model for coordinate transformation , 1990, 1990 IJCNN International Joint Conference on Neural Networks.

[165]  E. Chekaluk,et al.  Visual stimulus input, saccadic suppression, and detection of information from the postsaccade scene , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[166]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Visual memory and the perception of a stable visual environment , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[167]  K. Rayner,et al.  Role of spatial location in integration of pictorial information across saccades , 1990 .

[168]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Eye position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP and 7a of macaque , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[169]  R. Abrams,et al.  Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[170]  H Honda,et al.  The extraretinal signal from the pursuit-eye-movement system: Its role in the perceptual and the egocentric localization systems , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[171]  Mark E Nelson,et al.  Brain maps and parallel computers , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[172]  W. Haustein,et al.  Evaluation of retinal orientation and gaze direction in the perception of the vertical , 1990, Vision Research.

[173]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Neocortical memory circuits. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[174]  T. Brandt,et al.  Disorders of posture and gait. , 1990, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[175]  R. Andersen,et al.  Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. II. Spatial properties. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[176]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Texture and motion spreading, the aperture problem, and transparency , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[177]  E. Brenner Judging object motion during smooth pursuit eye movements: The role of optic flow , 1991, Vision Research.

[178]  A. Trehub The cognitive brain , 1991 .

[179]  L. Stark,et al.  Ocular proprioception and efference copy in registering visual direction , 1991, Vision Research.

[180]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Control of orienting gaze shifts by the tectoreticulospinal system in the head-free cat. III. Spatiotemporal characteristics of phasic motor discharges. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[181]  David E. Irwin Information integration across saccadic eye movements , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[182]  Lawrence Stark,et al.  Presbyopia research : from molecular biology to visual adaptation , 1991 .

[183]  Rodney A. Brooks,et al.  Intelligence Without Reason , 1991, IJCAI.

[184]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  Integration of Form across Saccadic Eye Movements , 1991, Perception.

[185]  J. Paillard Brain and space , 1991 .

[186]  I. Rentschler,et al.  Contrast thresholds for identification of numeric characters in direct and eccentric view , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[187]  D. E. Irwin Memory for position and identity across eye movements. , 1992 .

[188]  S Thorpe,et al.  Modulation of neural stereoscopic processing in primate area V1 by the viewing distance. , 1992, Science.

[189]  Dana H. Ballard,et al.  Principles of animate vision , 1992, CVGIP Image Underst..

[190]  D. Dennett,et al.  The Nature of Consciousness , 2006 .

[191]  K. Rayner Eye Movements and Visual Cognition , 1992 .

[192]  B. Bridgeman Conscious vs Unconscious Processes , 1992 .

[193]  L. Jami Golgi tendon organs in mammalian skeletal muscle: functional properties and central actions. , 1992, Physiological reviews.

[194]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[195]  E. Fetz Movement control: Are movement parameters recognizably coded in the activity of single neurons? , 1992 .

[196]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[197]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Visual response latencies in striate cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[198]  J. Stein The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex. , 1992, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[199]  P Dassonville,et al.  Oculomotor localization relies on a damped representation of saccadic eye displacement in human and nonhuman primates , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[200]  J. T. Enright The remarkable saccades of asymmetrical vergence , 1992, Vision Research.

[201]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Solving the "real" mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[202]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. , 1993, Science.

[203]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  How parallel are the primate visual pathways? , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[204]  B. Fischer,et al.  Express saccades and visual attention , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[205]  T. Hicks,et al.  The Visually Responsive Neuron: From Basic Neurophysiology to Behavior , 1993 .

[206]  H. Honda Saccade-contingent displacement of the apparent position of visual stimuli flashed on a dimly illuminated structured background , 1993, Vision Research.

[207]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Egocentric Spaw Representation in Early Vision , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[208]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Direction Constancy in the Oculomotor System , 1993 .

[209]  M Jüttner,et al.  Lateral information transfer across saccadic eye movements , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[210]  J. Malpeli,et al.  Responses of neurons in primary visual cortex are modulated by eye position. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[211]  Michael Jenkin,et al.  Spatial vision in humans and robots , 1994 .

[212]  E. Brenner,et al.  The difference between the perception of absolute and relative motion: a reaction time study , 1994, Vision Research.