Color Naming and the Phototoxic Effects of Sunlight on the Eye

Many languages have no basic color term for “blue.” Instead, they call short-wavelength stimuli “green” or “dark.” We show that this cultural, linguistic phenomenon could result from accelerated aging of the eye because of high, chronic exposure to ultraviolet-B (UV-B) in sunlight (e.g., phototoxic lens brunescence). Reviewing 203 world languages, we found a significant relationship between UV dosage and color naming: In low-UV localities, languages generally have the word “blue”; in high-UV areas, languages without “blue” prevail. Furthermore, speakers of these non-“blue” languages often show blue-yellow color vision deficiency. We tested our phototoxicity hypothesis in a color-naming experiment, using computerized, colorimetric simulations of Munsell colors as viewed through clear and brunescent lenses. As predicted, our young subjects used “blue” as in English when the simulated lens was clear, but named colors as in tropical languages when the lens was dense. Our within-subjects design precludes a cultural explanation for this result.

[1]  K. Jameson,et al.  Richer color experience in observers with multiple photopigment opsin genes , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  J. Davidoff,et al.  Color categories are not universal: replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  Gunther Wyszecki,et al.  Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition , 2000 .

[4]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Time course of chromatic adaptation for color appearance and discrimination , 2000, Vision Research.

[5]  Refractor Vision , 2000, The Lancet.

[6]  P. Kay,et al.  Color appearance and the emergence and evolution of basic color lexicons , 1999 .

[7]  SUPARNA DUTTASINHA,et al.  Magnet levitation at your fingertips (vol 400, pg 323, 1999) , 1999 .

[8]  J. Davidoff,et al.  Colour categories in a stone-age tribe , 1999, Nature.

[9]  Greville G. Corbett,et al.  Cross-cultural differences in colour vision: Acquired ‘colour-blindness’ in Africa , 1998 .

[10]  R. W. Casson,et al.  Color and Cognition in Mesoamerica: Constructing Categories as Vantages. , 1998 .

[11]  Robert E. Maclaury Color and Cognition in Mesoamerica: Constructing Categories as Vantages , 1997 .

[12]  B. Saunders,et al.  Are there nontrivial constraints on colour categorization? , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  B. Saunders,et al.  Colour: An exosomatic organ? , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  M D Fairchild,et al.  Time course of chromatic adaptation for color-appearance judgments. , 1995, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[15]  Paul T. Sowden,et al.  The basic colour terms of Chichewa , 1995 .

[16]  R. W. Young The family of sunlight-related eye diseases. , 1994, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[17]  Mark D. Fairchild,et al.  Chromatic adaptation to natural and incandescent illuminants , 1992, Vision Research.

[18]  J. Winderickx,et al.  Polymorphism in red photopigment underlies variation in colour matching , 1992, Nature.

[19]  J. Werner,et al.  Light, vision, and aging. , 1990, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[20]  J. Werner,et al.  Loss of human photoreceptor sensitivity associated with chronic exposure to ultraviolet radiation. , 1989, Ophthalmology.

[21]  F. Billmeyer Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd ed., by Gunter Wyszecki and W. S. Stiles, John Wiley and Sons, New York, 1982, 950 pp. Price: $75.00 , 1983 .

[22]  P. Kay,et al.  What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? , 1983 .

[23]  R. Weale,et al.  Congenital and Acquired Colour Vision Defects , 1981 .

[24]  P. Kay,et al.  The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms , 1978 .

[25]  飯沼 巌,et al.  The City University Colour Vision Testの紹介 , 1977 .

[26]  S. McKee,et al.  Quantitative studies in retinex theory a comparison between theoretical predictions and observer responses to the “color mondrian” experiments , 1976, Vision Research.

[27]  M. Bornstein Color vision and color naming: a psychophysiological hypothesis of cultural difference. , 1973, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  P. Kay,et al.  Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution , 1973 .

[29]  R. Harwerth,et al.  Prolonged Color Blindness Induced by Intense Spectral Lights in Rhesus Monkeys , 1971, Science.

[30]  W D Wright,et al.  Color Science, Concepts and Methods. Quantitative Data and Formulas , 1967 .

[31]  S. Ervin,et al.  Navaho color categories , 1960 .

[32]  D. B. Judd,et al.  Final Report of the O.S.A. Subcommittee on the Spacing of the Munsell Colors , 1943 .

[33]  A. C. Haddon,et al.  Reports of the Cambridge Anthropological Expedition to Torres Straits , 1936, Nature.

[34]  David L. Asher,et al.  Contribution to the History of the Development of the Human Race , 1881, Journal of Psychological Medicine and Mental Pathology (London, England : 1875).

[35]  Ernst Krause The Colour Sense , 1878, Nature.

[36]  Emre Özgen,et al.  Turkish color terms: tests of Berlin and Kay’s theory of color universals and linguistic relativity , 1998 .

[37]  P. Kay,et al.  Color categories in thought and language: Color naming across languages , 1997 .

[38]  G. Corbett,et al.  The basic color terms of Russian , 1994 .

[39]  H. McGurk,et al.  Color terms in Setswana: a linguistic and perceptual approach , 1992 .

[40]  F. Delori,et al.  Historic perspectives. Macular yellow pigment. The first 200 years. , 1981, Retina.

[41]  J. R. Wiggins The First 200 Years , 1976 .

[42]  G. Allen The Colour-Sense , 1878, Nature.