A reevaluation of the electrophysiological correlates of absolute pitch and relative pitch: No evidence for an absolute pitch-specific negativity.

Musicians with absolute pitch effortlessly identify the pitch of a sound without an external reference. Previous neuroscientific studies on absolute pitch have typically had small samples sizes and low statistical power, making them susceptible for false positive findings. In a seminal study, Itoh et al. (2005) reported the elicitation of an absolute pitch-specific event-related potential component during tone listening - the AP negativity. Additionally, they identified several components as correlates of relative pitch, the ability to identify relations between pitches. Here, we attempted to replicate the main findings of Itoh et al.'s study in a large sample of musicians (n = 104) using both frequentist and Bayesian inference. We were not able to replicate the presence of an AP negativity during tone listening in individuals with high levels of absolute pitch, but we partially replicated the findings concerning the correlates of relative pitch. Our results are consistent with several previous studies reporting an absence of differences between musicians with and without absolute pitch in early auditory evoked potential components. We conclude that replication studies form a crucial part in assessing extraordinary findings, even more so in small fields where a single finding can have a large impact on further research.

[1]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[2]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015 , 2018, Nature Human Behaviour.

[3]  E. Donchin,et al.  People with Absolute Pitch Process Tones Without Producing a P300 , 1984, Science.

[4]  Psyche Loui,et al.  Enhanced functional networks in absolute pitch , 2012, NeuroImage.

[5]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional anatomy of musical processing in listeners with absolute pitch and relative pitch. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Default Bayes factors for ANOVA designs , 2012 .

[7]  N. Kerr HARKing: Hypothesizing After the Results are Known , 1998, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[8]  R. Kakigi,et al.  One year of musical training affects development of auditory cortical-evoked fields in young children. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  Michael Bryce,et al.  Test 5.14.4. Deposit 18 June 15:43, embargoed 18/07/2019 : Article -> Review article , 2019 .

[10]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians , 1998, Nature.

[11]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science , 2015, Science.

[12]  Mante S. Nieuwland,et al.  Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension , 2018, eLife.

[13]  A. H. Takeuchi,et al.  Absolute pitch. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  An Empirical Reevaluation of Absolute Pitch: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Measurements , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  Josh H McDermott,et al.  Music Perception, Pitch, and the Auditory System This Review Comes from a Themed Issue on Sensory Systems Edited Pitch Relations across Time—relative Pitch Relative Pitch—behavioral Evidence Neural Mechanisms of Relative Pitch Representation of Simultaneous Pitches— Chords and Polyphony Summary and , 2022 .

[16]  Tsutomu Nakada,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of absolute pitch and relative pitch. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Neuroskeptic The Nine Circles of Scientific Hell , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[18]  B. McShane,et al.  Conceptualizing and evaluating replication across domains of behavioral research , 2018, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[19]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Electrical Neuroimaging: Imaging the electric neuronal generators of EEG/MEG , 2009 .

[20]  Jeffrey N Rouder,et al.  Bayesian Analysis of Factorial Designs , 2017, Psychological methods.

[21]  R. Frisina,et al.  Effects of musical training and absolute pitch ability on event-related activity in response to sine tones. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  H. Pashler,et al.  Puzzlingly High Correlations in fMRI Studies of Emotion, Personality, and Social Cognition 1 , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[23]  C. Begley,et al.  Drug development: Raise standards for preclinical cancer research , 2012, Nature.

[24]  M. Scherg,et al.  Morphology of Heschl's gyrus reflects enhanced activation in the auditory cortex of musicians , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Samantha F. Anderson,et al.  There's more than one way to conduct a replication study: Beyond statistical significance. , 2016, Psychological methods.

[26]  A. Bachem,et al.  Various Types of Absolute Pitch , 1937 .

[27]  Leif D. Nelson,et al.  False-Positive Psychology , 2011, Psychological science.

[28]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Hidden Multiplicity in Multiway ANOVA: Prevalence, Consequences, and Remedies , 2014 .

[29]  D. Simons The Value of Direct Replication , 2014, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[30]  R. Rosenthal The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results , 1979 .

[31]  J. Ioannidis Why Most Discovered True Associations Are Inflated , 2008, Epidemiology.

[32]  Joseph P. Walton,et al.  Effects of Musical Training and Absolute Pitch on the Neural Processing of Melodic Intervals: A P3 Event-Related Potential Study , 1992 .

[33]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[34]  J. Ioannidis,et al.  Empirical assessment of published effect sizes and power in the recent cognitive neuroscience and psychology literature , 2017, PLoS biology.

[35]  G. Schlaug,et al.  In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians , 1995, Science.

[36]  John P. A. Ioannidis,et al.  A manifesto for reproducible science , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.

[37]  N. Freimer,et al.  Absolute pitch: an approach for identification of genetic and nongenetic components. , 1998, American journal of human genetics.

[38]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Dichotomy and perceptual distortions in absolute pitch ability , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  A purely confirmatory replication study of structural brain-behavior correlations , 2015, Cortex.

[40]  Mathias S. Oechslin,et al.  Absolute Pitch—Functional Evidence of Speech-Relevant Auditory Acuity , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  Z. Dienes Bayesian Versus Orthodox Statistics: Which Side Are You On? , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[43]  D. Curran‐Everett,et al.  The fickle P value generates irreproducible results , 2015, Nature Methods.

[44]  Gottfried Schlaug,et al.  Perceiving pitch absolutely: Comparing absolute and relative pitch possessors in a pitch memory task , 2009, BMC Neuroscience.

[45]  Gideon Nave,et al.  Evaluating replicability of laboratory experiments in economics , 2016, Science.

[46]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  L. Garcia-Larrea,et al.  P3, positive slow wave and working memory load: a study on the functional correlates of slow wave activity. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  Carl T. Bergstrom,et al.  Publication bias and the canonization of false facts , 2016, eLife.

[50]  A. Heinecke,et al.  Increased volume and function of right auditory cortex as a marker for absolute pitch. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: a problem of significance , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  H. C. Li,et al.  Enhanced Cortical Connectivity in Absolute Pitch Musicians: A Model for Local Hyperconnectivity , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  M. Annett A classification of hand preference by association analysis. , 1970, British journal of psychology.

[54]  L. Jäncke,et al.  Electrophysiological Correlates of Absolute Pitch in a Passive Auditory Oddball Paradigm: a Direct Replication Attempt , 2018, eNeuro.

[55]  H. Jeffreys,et al.  Theory of probability , 1896 .

[56]  Diana Deutsch,et al.  5 – Absolute Pitch , 2013 .

[57]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[58]  A. Gelman,et al.  The Difference Between “Significant” and “Not Significant” is not Itself Statistically Significant , 2006 .

[59]  Nicholas Gaspelin,et al.  How to get statistically significant effects in any ERP experiment (and why you shouldn't). , 2017, Psychophysiology.

[60]  M. Baker 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility , 2016, Nature.

[61]  Camilla L. Nord,et al.  Power-up: A Reanalysis of 'Power Failure' in Neuroscience Using Mixture Modeling , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  Mikko Sams,et al.  Absolute Pitch and Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1993 .

[63]  G. Schlaug,et al.  Absolute Pitch and Planum Temporale , 2001, NeuroImage.

[64]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Absolute Pitch: Evidence for Early Cognitive Facilitation during Passive Listening as Revealed by Reduced P3a Amplitudes , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.