Modeling the Interactions of Bottom-Up and Top-Down Guidance in Visual Attention
暂无分享,去创建一个
Thomas Geerinck | David Henderickx | Eric Soetens | Kathleen Maetens | E. Soetens | David Henderickx | Thomas Geerinck | Kathleen Maetens
[1] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.
[2] P. Goldman-Rakic. Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.
[3] John P. Aggleton,et al. The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction. , 1992 .
[4] Hichem Sahli,et al. Modeling Attention and Perceptual Grouping to Salient Objects , 2008, WAPCV.
[5] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] C. Koch,et al. Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[7] M. Gabriel,et al. Functions of anterior and posterior cingulate cortex during avoidance learning in rabbits. , 1990, Progress in brain research.
[8] Leslie G. Ungerleider. Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .
[9] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[10] Charles L. Folk,et al. Can new objects override attentional control settings? , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] M. Corbetta,et al. A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[12] A. Treisman,et al. Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] R W Remington,et al. The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and performance IX , 1982 .
[15] C. Eriksen,et al. Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays , 1972 .
[16] R. Remington. Attention and saccadic eye movements. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] R. Rescorla. Facilitation and inhibition. , 1987 .
[18] D. J. Felleman,et al. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.
[19] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] R. Mansfield,et al. Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .
[21] J. Allman,et al. Mapping human visual cortex with positron emission tomography , 1986, Nature.
[22] K. Briand,et al. Feature integration and spatial attention : More evidence of a dissociation between endogenous and exogenous orienting , 1998 .
[23] P. Goldman-Rakic,et al. Preface: Cerebral Cortex Has Come of Age , 1991 .
[24] S. Dornič,et al. Attention and performance V , 1976 .
[25] M. Posner,et al. Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .
[26] R. Remington,et al. Top-down modulation of preattentive processing: Testing the recovery account of contingent capture , 2006 .
[27] M. Posner,et al. The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.
[28] A. Treisman,et al. Visual memory for novel shapes: implicit coding without attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[29] E. Soetens,et al. Feature integration and spatial attention: common processes for endogenous and exogenous orienting , 2010, Psychological research.
[30] Jan Theeuwes,et al. The relationship between exogenous and endogenous saccades and attention. , 2003 .
[31] R. Klein,et al. Perceptual-motor expectancies interact with covert visual orienting under conditions of endogenous but not exogenous control. , 1994, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[32] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[33] R. Remington,et al. Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 4.0: Current Progress With a Model of Visual Search , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.
[36] C. Koch,et al. A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.
[37] P. T. Fox,et al. Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing , 1988, Nature.
[38] D. LaBerge,et al. Positron emission tomographic measurements of pulvinar activity during an attention task , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[39] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[40] James J. Clark. Spatial attention and latencies of saccadic eye movements , 1999, Vision Research.
[41] S. Petersen,et al. The pulvinar and visual salience , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[42] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[43] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[44] S. Yantis,et al. Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.