Articulatory duration in single-word speech production.

Three different speech production paradigms assessed C. T. Kello, D. C. Plaut, and B. MacWhinney's (2000) claim that the characteristics of speech production flexibly vary between staged and cascaded modes depending on task demand. All experiments measured response latencies and durations of single words without and with a response deadline. Experiment 1 used a picture-word interference task; Experiment 2 blocked pictures either by semantic category or by word-initial overlap; and Experiment 3 used a Stroop paradigm. In all cases, systematic effects of semantic and form relatedness were obtained on latencies but not on response durations. These results support the assumption that articulation, as assessed by response duration, is never influenced by central cognitive processes once a response has been initiated.

[1]  T. D. Hanley,et al.  Effect of level of distracting noise upon speaking rate, duration and intensity. , 1949, The Journal of speech disorders.

[2]  R. Golinkoff,et al.  Automatic semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. , 1975 .

[3]  M. F. Garrett,et al.  The Analysis of Sentence Production1 , 1975 .

[4]  K. Rayner,et al.  Visual-feature and response components in a picture-word interference task with beginning and skilled readers. , 1977, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  A. Crompton,et al.  Syllables and segments in speech production , 1981 .

[6]  A. Cutler Slips of the Tongue and Language Production , 1982 .

[7]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[8]  R. H. Bernacki,et al.  Effects of noise on speech production: acoustic and perceptual analyses. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Cascade processes in picture identification , 1988 .

[10]  N. Martin,et al.  Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors: Implications for models of lexical retrieval☆ , 1989 .

[11]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[12]  Julie E. Boland,et al.  Priming in pronunciation: Beyond pattern recognition and onset latency , 1989 .

[13]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .

[14]  A. Meyer The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word ☆ , 1990 .

[15]  D. Whalen Coarticulation is largely planned , 1990 .

[16]  H. Schriefers,et al.  Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli. , 1991 .

[17]  A. Meyer The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable , 1991 .

[18]  D. Balota,et al.  Repetition and Associative Context Effects in Speech Production , 1991, Language and speech.

[19]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming , 1991 .

[20]  G. Dell,et al.  Mediated and convergent lexical priming in language production: a comment on Levelt et al. (1991). , 1991, Psychological review.

[21]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[22]  J C Junqua,et al.  The Lombard reflex and its role on human listeners and automatic speech recognizers. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  Phonological activation of semantic competitors during lexical access in speech production , 1993 .

[24]  W. Levelt,et al.  Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary? , 1994, Cognition.

[25]  J. Kroll,et al.  Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connections Between Bilingual Memory Representations , 1994 .

[26]  A. Roelofs,et al.  Serial Order in Planning the Production of Successive Morphemes of a Word , 1996 .

[27]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  A. Roelofs,et al.  The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production , 1997, Cognition.

[29]  P. Hagoort,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence on the time course of semantic and phonological processes in speech production. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence on the time course of semantic and phonological processes in speech production. , 1997 .

[31]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Stroop-Type Interference: Congruity Effects in Color Naming With Typewritten Responses , 1998 .

[32]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Rightward incrementality in encoding simple phrasal forms in speech production : Verb-particle combinations , 1998 .

[33]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Initial phoneme versus whole-word criterion to initiate pronunciation: Evidence based on response latency and initial phoneme duration. , 1998 .

[34]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Runword: An IBM-PC software package for the collection and acoustic analysis of speeded naming responses , 1998 .

[35]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Phonological Segments and Features as Planning Units in Speech Production , 1999 .

[36]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Parallel processing and initial phoneme criterion in naming words : Evidence from frequency effects on onset and rime duration , 1999 .

[37]  H. Schriefers Phonological Facilitation in the Production of Two-word Utterances , 1999 .

[38]  H. Schriefers,et al.  Parallelism in the Production of Noun Phrases: Experiments and Reaction Time Models. , 1999 .

[39]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  D. Plaut,et al.  The task dependence of staged versus cascaded processing: an empirical and computational study of Stroop interference in speech production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[41]  P. O'Seaghdha,et al.  Phonological competition and cooperation in form-related priming: sequential and nonsequential processes in word production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Strategic control in word reading: evidence from speeded responding in the tempo-naming task. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  W. Levelt,et al.  Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words , 2001, Cognition.

[44]  Dirk P. Janssen,et al.  Inflectional frames in language production , 2002 .

[45]  R. Hartsuiker,et al.  The interplay of meaning, sound, and syntax in sentence production. , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[46]  W. Levelt,et al.  Semantic Category Interference in Overt Picture Naming: Sharpening Current Density Localization by PCA , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  A. Roelofs,et al.  Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. , 2003, Psychological review.

[48]  Molly Babel,et al.  The sequential cueing effect in children's speech production , 2005, Applied Psycholinguistics.