No Evidence that Working Memory Modulates the Plasticity of the Nociceptive System, as Measured by Secondary Mechanical Hypersensitivity.

[1]  A. J. Palmer,et al.  The effect of high versus low cognitive load on the development of nociceptive hypersensitivity: The roles of sympathetic arousal, sex and pain‐related fear , 2023, European journal of pain.

[2]  A. Mouraux,et al.  No evidence for an effect of selective spatial attention on the development of secondary hyperalgesia: A replication study , 2022, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.

[3]  A. Meulders,et al.  The effect of observing high or low pain on the development of central sensitization. , 2022, The journal of pain.

[4]  Mariella Pazzaglia,et al.  Heart Rate Variability and Pain: A Systematic Review , 2022, Brain sciences.

[5]  I. Blanchette,et al.  Disruption of working memory and contralateral delay activity by nociceptive stimuli is modulated by task demands. , 2021, Pain.

[6]  I. Blanchette,et al.  Reduction of pain and spinal nociceptive transmission by working memory is load dependant. , 2021, The journal of pain.

[7]  P. Rainville,et al.  Inter-individual predictors of pain inhibition during performance of a competing cognitive task , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[8]  A. Khatibi,et al.  Attentional control moderates the relationship between pain catastrophizing and selective attention to pain faces on the antisaccade task , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[9]  M. Tegenthoff,et al.  Distraction by a cognitive task has a higher impact on electrophysiological measures compared with conditioned pain modulation , 2020, BMC neuroscience.

[10]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Within- and between-session reliability of secondary hyperalgesia induced by electrical high-frequency stimulation. , 2020, European journal of pain.

[11]  A. R. Otto,et al.  Forced choices reveal a trade-off between cognitive effort and physical pain , 2020, eLife.

[12]  P. Pauli,et al.  Fear of pain and pain intensity: Meta-analysis and systematic review. , 2020, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  A. von Leupoldt,et al.  A highly cognitive demanding working memory task may prevent the development of nociceptive hypersensitivity. , 2020, Pain.

[14]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Burst-like conditioning electrical stimulation is more efficacious than continuous stimulation for inducing secondary hyperalgesia in humans. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  A. Mouraux,et al.  The focus of spatial attention during the induction of central sensitization can modulate the subsequent development of secondary hyperalgesia , 2019, Cortex.

[16]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Heterosynaptic facilitation of mechanical nociceptive input is dependent on the frequency of conditioning stimulation. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  J. Deharo,et al.  Mental Workload Alters Heart Rate Variability, Lowering Non-linear Dynamics , 2019, Front. Physiol..

[18]  I. Blanchette,et al.  Improving working memory and pain inhibition in older persons using transcranial direct current stimulation , 2019, Neuroscience Research.

[19]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Quickly responding C‐fibre nociceptors contribute to heat hypersensitivity in the area of secondary hyperalgesia , 2018, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  S. Laborde,et al.  Vagal Tank Theory: The Three Rs of Cardiac Vagal Control Functioning – Resting, Reactivity, and Recovery , 2018, Front. Neurosci..

[21]  V. Legrain,et al.  No perceptual prioritization of non-nociceptive vibrotactile and visual stimuli presented on a sensitized body part , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[22]  I. Blanchette,et al.  Enhancement of pain inhibition by working memory with anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex , 2018, The Journal of Physiological Sciences.

[23]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models , 2017 .

[24]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Phase-locked and non-phase-locked EEG responses to pinprick stimulation before and after experimentally-induced secondary hyperalgesia , 2017, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[25]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Attention to pain! A neurocognitive perspective on attentional modulation of pain in neuroimaging studies , 2017, Cortex.

[26]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Intense pain influences the cortical processing of visual stimuli projected onto the sensitized skin , 2017, Pain.

[27]  J. B. Holzman,et al.  Heart rate variability indices as bio-markers of top-down self-regulatory mechanisms: A meta-analytic review , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[28]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Central Sensitization of Mechanical Nociceptive Pathways Is Associated with a Long-Lasting Increase of Pinprick-Evoked Brain Potentials , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[29]  Matthias J. Wieser,et al.  Attention mechanisms during predictable and unpredictable threat — A steady-state visual evoked potential approach , 2016, NeuroImage.

[30]  D. Sanabria,et al.  Heart rate variability and cognitive processing: The autonomic response to task demands , 2016, Biological Psychology.

[31]  P. Pauli,et al.  Sustained attention in context conditioning: Evidence from steady-state VEPs. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[32]  J. Woo,et al.  Reliability of ultra-short-term analysis as a surrogate of standard 5-min analysis of heart rate variability. , 2015, Telemedicine journal and e-health : the official journal of the American Telemedicine Association.

[33]  Ulrike Bingel,et al.  Phasic and Tonic Pain Differentially Impact the Interruptive Function of Pain , 2015, PloS one.

[34]  A. Flatt,et al.  Ultra-short-term heart rate variability indexes at rest and post-exercise in athletes: evaluating the agreement with accepted recommendations. , 2014, Journal of sports science & medicine.

[35]  A. Mouraux,et al.  High-frequency electrical stimulation of the human skin induces heterotopical mechanical hyperalgesia, heat hyperalgesia, and enhanced responses to nonnociceptive vibrotactile input. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Jerker Rönnberg,et al.  Individual differences in distractibility: An update and a model , 2014, PsyCh journal.

[37]  M. Osaka,et al.  The influence of working memory capacity on experimental heat pain. , 2013, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[38]  K. Davis,et al.  Neural underpinnings of behavioural strategies that prioritize either cognitive task performance or pain , 2013, PAIN®.

[39]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Shielding cognition from nociception with working memory , 2013, Cortex.

[40]  Antonio Luque-Casado,et al.  Cognitive Performance and Heart Rate Variability: The Influence of Fitness Level , 2013, PloS one.

[41]  Alexander M. Nitsch,et al.  Interaction between stimulus intensity and perceptual load in the attentional control of pain , 2013, PAIN®.

[42]  Stefan Stenfelt,et al.  Working Memory Capacity and Visual–Verbal Cognitive Load Modulate Auditory–Sensory Gating in the Brainstem: Toward a Unified View of Attention , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  H. van Goor,et al.  The effect of high-frequency conditioning stimulation of human skin on reported pain intensity and event-related potentials. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Christian Büchel,et al.  Attention Modulates Spinal Cord Responses to Pain , 2012, Current Biology.

[45]  G. Asmundson Do attentional biases for pain depend on threat value of pain and competing motivation toward non-pain goals? , 2012, PAIN.

[46]  André Mouraux,et al.  Steady-state evoked potentials to tag specific components of nociceptive cortical processing , 2012, NeuroImage.

[47]  Jessie Y. C. Chen,et al.  Supervisory Control of Multiple Robots , 2012, Hum. Factors.

[48]  B. Oken,et al.  Sensitivity to mental effort and test–retest reliability of heart rate variability measures in healthy seniors , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[49]  K. Verhoeven,et al.  Distraction from pain and executive functioning: An experimental investigation of the role of inhibition, task switching and working memory , 2011, European journal of pain.

[50]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Controlling Attention to Nociceptive Stimuli with Working Memory , 2011, PloS one.

[51]  A. Keil,et al.  Temporal Trade-Off Effects in Sustained Attention: Dynamics in Visual Cortex Predict the Target Detection Performance during Distraction , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  André Mouraux,et al.  The role of working memory in the attentional control of pain , 2011, PAIN®.

[53]  C. Sanchez Working through the pain: Working memory capacity and differences in processing and storage under pain , 2011, Memory.

[54]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  The Relationship between N-Back Performance and Matrix Reasoning--Implications for Training and Transfer. , 2010 .

[55]  Patrik Sörqvist,et al.  High working memory capacity attenuates the deviation effect but not the changing-state effect: Further support for the duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction , 2010, Memory & cognition.

[56]  Sidney J Segalowitz,et al.  Autonomic predictors of Stroop performance in young and middle-aged adults. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[57]  James N. Campbell,et al.  Catastrophizing delays the analgesic effect of distraction , 2010, PAIN®.

[58]  T. Wager,et al.  Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task , 2010, PAIN®.

[59]  L. Arendt-Nielsen,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of nociceptive heterosynaptic long-term potentiation in humans. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  Caroline Di Bernardi Luft,et al.  Heart rate variability and cognitive function: Effects of physical effort , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[61]  K. Davis,et al.  A neurocognitive model of attention to pain: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence , 2009, PAIN®.

[62]  J. Thayer,et al.  Heart Rate Variability, Prefrontal Neural Function, and Cognitive Performance: The Neurovisceral Integration Perspective on Self-regulation, Adaptation, and Health , 2009, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[63]  N. Werner,et al.  Relationships between features of autonomic cardiovascular control and cognitive performance , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[64]  G. Crombez,et al.  Coping with pain: A motivational perspective , 2008, PAIN.

[65]  Myeong Gi Jeong,et al.  Ultra Short Term Analysis of Heart Rate Variability for Monitoring Mental Stress in Mobile Settings , 2007, 2007 29th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[66]  E. Wagenmakers A practical solution to the pervasive problems ofp values , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[67]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Are laser-evoked brain potentials modulated by attending to first or second pain? , 2007, Pain.

[68]  Karen D Davis,et al.  Interactions of pain intensity and cognitive load: the brain stays on task. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[69]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. , 2007, Emotion.

[70]  S. Porges The polyvagal perspective , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[71]  S. George,et al.  Fear of pain, not pain catastrophizing, predicts acute pain intensity, but neither factor predicts tolerance or blood pressure reactivity: An experimental investigation in pain‐free individuals , 2006 .

[72]  E. Fox,et al.  Anxiety modulates the degree of attentive resources required to process emotional faces , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[73]  V. Legrain,et al.  Involuntary orientation of attention to unattended deviant nociceptive stimuli is modulated by concomitant visual task difficulty. Evidence from laser evoked potentials , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[74]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[75]  K. D. Davis,et al.  Cognitive modulation of pain-related brain responses depends on behavioral strategy , 2004, Pain.

[76]  E. Viding,et al.  Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[77]  Till Sprenger,et al.  Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain—an fMRI analysis , 2004, Pain.

[78]  W. Maixner,et al.  Catastrophizing predicts changes in thermal pain responses after resolution of acute dental pain. , 2004, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[79]  A. K. Blangsted,et al.  The effect of mental stress on heart rate variability and blood pressure during computer work , 2004, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[80]  G. Crombez,et al.  The role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear in vigilance to pain: a structural equations approach , 2004, Pain.

[81]  J. Sandkühler,et al.  Perceptual Correlates of Nociceptive Long-term Potentiation and Long-term Depression in Humans , 2003 .

[82]  Jonathan Downar,et al.  Neural correlates of the prolonged salience of painful stimulation , 2003, NeuroImage.

[83]  J. Thayer,et al.  Vagal influence on working memory and attention. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[84]  D. Derryberry,et al.  Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. , 2002, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[85]  G. Crombez,et al.  A confirmatory factor analysis of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale: invariant factor structure across clinical and non-clinical populations , 2002, Pain.

[86]  S. Clare,et al.  Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[87]  S. Porges The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[88]  E. Keogh,et al.  Selective attentional bias for pain-related stimuli amongst pain fearful individuals , 2001, Pain.

[89]  R. Lane,et al.  A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.

[90]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[91]  S. Stone-Elander,et al.  Pain-related cerebral activation is altered by a distracting cognitive task , 2000, Pain.

[92]  G. Crombez,et al.  Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[93]  D. McNeil,et al.  Development of the Fear of Pain Questionnaire-III , 1998, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[94]  G. Crombez,et al.  Attentional disruption is enhanced by the threat of pain. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[95]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Verbal Working Memory Load Affects Regional Brain Activation as Measured by PET , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[96]  S. Porges Orienting in a defensive world: mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. A Polyvagal Theory. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[97]  R W Backs,et al.  Metabolic and cardiorespiratory measures of mental effort: the effects of level of difficulty in a working memory task. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[98]  P. Jorna Spectral analysis of heart rate and psychological state: A review of its validity as a workload index , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[99]  H. Bozdogan Model selection and Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC): The general theory and its analytical extensions , 1987 .

[100]  J Aasman,et al.  Operator Effort and the Measurement of Heart-Rate Variability , 1987, Human factors.

[101]  L. Mulder,et al.  Information processing and cardiovascular control. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[102]  Daniel G Bobrow,et al.  On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[103]  Mika P. Tarvainen,et al.  Kubios HRV - Heart rate variability analysis software , 2014, Comput. Methods Programs Biomed..

[104]  J. Kenemans,et al.  Pain and attention: attentional disruption or distraction? , 2006, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[105]  J. Vlaeyen,et al.  Is pain-related fear a predictor of somatosensory hypervigilance in chronic low back pain patients? , 2002, Behaviour research and therapy.

[106]  L. McCracken “Attention” to pain in persons with chronic pain: A behavioral approach , 1997 .

[107]  Scott R. Bishop,et al.  The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and validation. , 1995 .

[108]  J. Veltman,et al.  Indices of mental workload in a complex task environment. , 1993, Neuropsychobiology.