Optimality and Some of Its Discontents: Successes and Shortcomings of Existing Models for Binary Decisions
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Catalin V. Buhusi,et al. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[2] Angela J. Yu,et al. Should I stay or should I go? How the human brain manages the trade-off between exploitation and exploration , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[3] R. Ratcliff,et al. A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[4] James L. McClelland,et al. Integration of Sensory and Reward Information during Perceptual Decision-Making in Lateral Intraparietal Cortex (LIP) of the Macaque Monkey , 2010, PloS one.
[5] Han L J van der Maas,et al. Optimal decision making in neural inhibition models. , 2012, Psychological review.
[6] J. Gibbon. Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .
[7] James L. McClelland,et al. A parallel distributed processing approach to automaticity. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.
[8] James L. McClelland,et al. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .
[9] Donald Laming,et al. Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .
[10] M. Stone. Models for choice-reaction time , 1960 .
[11] M. Shadlen,et al. Representation of Confidence Associated with a Decision by Neurons in the Parietal Cortex , 2009, Science.
[12] Kenneth D. Miller,et al. Mathematical Equivalence of Two Common Forms of Firing Rate Models of Neural Networks , 2012, Neural Computation.
[13] P. Rabbitt. Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[14] Philip Holmes,et al. Simple Neural Networks that Optimize Decisions , 2005, Int. J. Bifurc. Chaos.
[15] J. Wolfowitz,et al. Optimum Character of the Sequential Probability Ratio Test , 1948 .
[16] Xiao-Jing Wang,et al. A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] Philip Holmes,et al. Can Monkeys Choose Optimally When Faced with Noisy Stimuli and Unequal Rewards? , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[18] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.
[19] W. Todd Maddox,et al. On the generality of optimal versus objective classifier feedback effects on decision criterion learning in perceptual categorization , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[20] K. H. Britten,et al. Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.
[21] Joshua W. Brown,et al. Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.
[22] Naomi Ehrich Leonard,et al. Can Post-Error Dynamics Explain Sequential Reaction Time Patterns? , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[23] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[24] Angela J. Yu,et al. Uncertainty, Neuromodulation, and Attention , 2005, Neuron.
[25] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.
[26] Andrew M. Saxe,et al. Acquisition of decision making criteria: reward rate ultimately beats accuracy , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[27] James L. McClelland,et al. Dynamic Integration of Reward and Stimulus Information in Perceptual Decision-Making , 2011, PloS one.
[28] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. An exploration-exploitation model based on norepinepherine and dopamine activity , 2005, NIPS.
[29] Philip Holmes,et al. Rapid decision threshold modulation by reward rate in a neural network , 2006, Neural Networks.
[30] C. Eriksen,et al. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] E. Wagenmakers,et al. A diffusion model decomposition of the practice effect , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[32] C. Koch,et al. Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalry , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[33] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Do Humans Produce the Speed–accuracy Trade-off That Maximizes Reward Rate? , 2022 .
[34] Angela J. Yu,et al. Dynamics of attentional selection under conflict: toward a rational Bayesian account. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[36] G. Logan,et al. When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .
[37] J. Gold,et al. Banburismus and the Brain Decoding the Relationship between Sensory Stimuli, Decisions, and Reward , 2002, Neuron.
[38] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Reward rate optimization in two-alternative decision making: empirical tests of theoretical predictions. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[39] J D Cohen,et al. A network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. , 1990, Science.
[40] Anders Ledberg,et al. Neurobiological Models of Two-Choice Decision Making Can Be Reduced to a One-Dimensional Nonlinear Diffusion Equation , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[41] Jerome R. Busemeyer,et al. Criterion Learning in a Deferred Decision-Making Task , 1989 .
[42] J. Cohen,et al. The role of locus coeruleus in the regulation of cognitive performance. , 1999, Science.
[43] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. A computational model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response tasks: Effects of frequency, sequence, and conflict , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[44] Sander Nieuwenhuis,et al. Pupil Diameter Predicts Changes in the Exploration–Exploitation Trade-off: Evidence for the Adaptive Gain Theory , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[45] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Mechanisms underlying dependencies of performance on stimulus history in a two-alternative forced-choice task , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[46] Philip Holmes,et al. A Neural Network Model of the Eriksen Task: Reduction, Analysis, and Data Fitting , 2008, Neural Computation.
[47] Philip L. Smith,et al. Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[48] Joseph T. McGuire,et al. Effort discounting in human nucleus accumbens , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[49] Jeffrey D. Schall,et al. Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[50] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The Computational and Neural Basis of Cognitive Control: Charted Territory and New Frontiers , 2014, Cogn. Sci..
[51] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[52] J. Gibbon,et al. Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[53] R. O’Connell,et al. Pupillometry and P3 index the locus coeruleus-noradrenergic arousal function in humans. , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[54] Woods Gordon. The Acquisition Decision , 1985 .
[55] Eric Shea-Brown,et al. Optimization of Decision Making in Multilayer Networks: The Role of Locus Coeruleus , 2008, Neural Computation.
[56] Philip Holmes,et al. Dynamical Analysis of Bayesian Inference Models for the Eriksen Task , 2009, Neural Computation.
[57] R. Ratcliff,et al. Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time. , 1999, Psychological review.
[58] J. Townsend,et al. Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.
[59] Mark S. Gilzenrat,et al. Pupil diameter tracks changes in control state predicted by the adaptive gain theory of locus coeruleus function , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[60] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Sequential effects: Superstition or rational behavior? , 2008, NIPS.
[61] G. Kane. Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, vol 1: Foundations, vol 2: Psychological and Biological Models , 1994 .
[62] M Zacksenhouse,et al. Robust versus optimal strategies for two-alternative forced choice tasks. , 2010, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[63] Philip Holmes,et al. Optimality and Robustness of a Biophysical Decision-Making Model under Norepinephrine Modulation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[64] W. Newsome,et al. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[65] Philip Holmes,et al. Dimension Reduction and Dynamics of a Spiking Neural Network Model for Decision Making under Neuromodulation , 2011, SIAM J. Appl. Dyn. Syst..
[66] M. Shadlen,et al. Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[67] J. Gold,et al. Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[68] D. Laming. Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .
[69] Lesley F. Wright,et al. Information Gap Decision Theory: Decisions under Severe Uncertainty , 2004 .
[70] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Nonlinear neural networks: Principles, mechanisms, and architectures , 1988, Neural Networks.
[71] Philip L. Smith,et al. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[72] James L. McClelland,et al. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.
[73] Paul R. Cohen,et al. ON THE CONTROL OF AUTOMATIC PROCESSES : A PARALLEL DISTRIBUTED PROCESSING MODEL OF THE STROOP EFFECT Technical Report AIP-40 , 2015 .
[74] Roger Ratcliff,et al. Dissociable Perceptual-learning Mechanisms Revealed by Diffusion-model Analysis , 2022 .
[75] M. Shadlen,et al. A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[76] Corey J. Bohil,et al. Base-rate and payoff effects in multidimensional perceptual categorization. , 1998, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.
[77] James L. McClelland,et al. The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.
[78] KongFatt Wong-Lin,et al. Sequential Effects in Two-Choice Reaction Time Tasks: Decomposition and Synthesis of Mechanisms , 2009, Neural Computation.
[79] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.
[80] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.
[81] M. Posner,et al. Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .
[82] Timothy D. Hanks,et al. Elapsed Decision Time Affects the Weighting of Prior Probability in a Perceptual Decision Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[83] L. Jacoby,et al. Stroop process dissociations: the relationship between facilitation and interference. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[84] Francis Tuerlinckx,et al. Do the Dynamics of Prior Information Depend on Task Context? An Analysis of Optimal Performance and an Empirical Test , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[85] Alan S. Brown,et al. Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .