How People with Low and High Graph Literacy Process Health Graphs: Evidence from Eye‐tracking

Graphs facilitate the communication of important quantitative information, often serving as effective decision support tools. Yet, graphs are not equally useful for all individuals, as people differ substantially in their graph literacy—the ability to understand graphically presented information. Although some features of graphs can be interpreted using spatial-to-conceptual mappings that can be established by adults and children with no graphing experience (e.g., “higher bars equal larger quantities”), other features are linked to arbitrary graph conventions (e.g., axes labels and scales). In two experiments, we examined differences in the processes underlying the comprehension of graphs presenting medical information in individuals with low and high graph literacy. Participants' eye movements were recorded while they interpreted graphs in which information in conventional features was incongruent with that conveyed by spatial features. Results revealed that participants with low graph literacy more often relied on misleading spatial-to-conceptual mappings and misinterpreted the data depicted. Higher graph literacy was often associated with more time spent viewing the conventional features containing essential information for accurate interpretations. This suggests that individuals with high graph literacy are better able to identify the task-relevant information in graphs, and thus attend to the relevant features to a larger extent. Theoretical, methodological, and prescriptive implications for customization of decision-support systems are discussed.

[1]  Bracha Kramarski,et al.  Enhancing Mathematical Reasoning in the Classroom: The Effects of Cooperative Learning and Metacognitive Training , 2003 .

[2]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: A protocol analysis and process model evaluation , 2009, Judgment and Decision Making.

[3]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Conceptual limitations in comprehending line graphs. , 1995 .

[4]  Michael Siegrist,et al.  Numeric and graphic risk information processing of high and low numerates in the intuitive and deliberative decision modes: An eye-tracker study , 2014, Judgment and Decision Making.

[5]  H. Haider,et al.  Eye movement during skill acquisition: More evidence for the information-reduction hypothesis , 1999 .

[6]  Dirk P. Janssen,et al.  Twice random, once mixed: Applying mixed models to simultaneously analyze random effects of language and participants , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.

[7]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Effective communication of risks to young adults: using message framing and visual aids to increase condom use and STD screening. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[8]  M. Galesic,et al.  Graph Literacy , 2011, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[9]  Raj M. Ratwani,et al.  Shedding light on the graph schema: Perceptual features versus invariant structure , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Individual Differences in Graph Literacy: Overcoming Denominator Neglect in Risk Comprehension , 2012 .

[11]  Robert McGill,et al.  An Experiment in Graphical Perception , 1986, Int. J. Man Mach. Stud..

[12]  David K. Simkin,et al.  An Information-Processing Analysis of Graph Perception , 1987 .

[13]  Wind Cowles,et al.  Investigating graph comprehension in students with dyslexia: an eye tracking study. , 2014, Research in developmental disabilities.

[14]  R. Ratcliff Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  M. Jones,et al.  The Use and Abuse of Graphs in Annual Reports: Theoretical Framework and Empirical Study , 1992 .

[16]  B. Rimer,et al.  General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[17]  A. Meade,et al.  Identifying careless responses in survey data. , 2012, Psychological methods.

[18]  Peter C.-H. Cheng,et al.  Modeling the Effect of Task and Graphical Representation on Response Latency in a Graph Reading Task , 2003, Hum. Factors.

[19]  M. Siegrist,et al.  How do people perceive graphical risk communication? The role of subjective numeracy , 2011 .

[20]  Carmen Keller,et al.  Using a Familiar Risk Comparison Within a Risk Ladder to Improve Risk Understanding by Low Numerates: A Study of Visual Attention , 2011, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[21]  Rocio Garcia-Retamero,et al.  Communicating and Distorting Risks with Graphs: An Eye-Tracking Study , 2012 .

[22]  Jacob L. Orquin,et al.  Learning affects top down and bottom up modulation of eye movements in decision making , 2013, Judgment and Decision Making.

[23]  M. Ashcraft,et al.  The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Mapping conceptual to spatial relations in visual reasoning. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  Peter A. Frensch,et al.  The Role of Information Reduction in Skill Acquisition , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  The Influence of Skills, Message Frame, and Visual Aids on Prevention of Sexually Transmitted Diseases , 2014 .

[27]  Priti Shah,et al.  The Comprehension of Quantitative Information in Graphical Displays. , 2005 .

[28]  Mirta Galesic,et al.  When Higher Bars Are Not Larger Quantities: On Individual Differences in the Use of Spatial Information in Graph Comprehension , 2012, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[29]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Science Current Directions in Psychological , 2010 .

[30]  M. Gattis Structure mapping in spatial reasoning , 2002 .

[31]  Priti Shah,et al.  Bar and Line Graph Comprehension: An Interaction of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Processes , 2011, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[32]  Aidan Feeney,et al.  Individual Differences in Graphical Reasoning , 2004, Diagrams.

[33]  M. Galesic,et al.  Who profits from visual aids: overcoming challenges in people's understanding of risks [corrected]. , 2010, Social science & medicine.

[34]  Toru Ishikawa,et al.  Landmark Selection in the Environment: Relationships with Object Characteristics and Sense of Direction , 2012, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[35]  Vladislav J. Mikulich,et al.  The quantity and quality of scientific graphs in pharmaceutical advertisements , 2003, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[36]  Merideth Gattis,et al.  Mapping relational structure in spatial reasoning , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[37]  Vivien Beattie,et al.  Measurement distortion of graphs in corporate reports: an experimental study , 2002 .

[38]  Edward T. Cokely,et al.  Measuring Risk Literacy: The Berlin Numeracy Test , 2012, Judgment and Decision Making.

[39]  I. Lipkus Numeric, Verbal, and Visual Formats of Conveying Health Risks: Suggested Best Practices and Future Recommendations , 2007, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[40]  Lynn Huestegge,et al.  Effects of spatial compatibility on integration processes in graph comprehension , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[41]  S. Kosslyn Understanding charts and graphs , 1989 .

[42]  B. Tversky,et al.  Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[43]  Priti Shah,et al.  A Model of the Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Graph Comprehension , 1998 .

[44]  E. Peters Beyond Comprehension , 2012 .

[45]  Jessica S. Ancker,et al.  The Practice of Informatics: Design Features of Graphs in Health Risk Communication: A Systematic Review , 2006, J. Am. Medical Informatics Assoc..

[46]  U. Maichle Chapter 13 Cognitive Processes in Understanding Line Graphs , 1994 .

[47]  S. Sutton,et al.  One-week recall of health risk information and individual differences in attention to bar charts , 2014 .

[48]  Gerald L. Lohse,et al.  A Cognitive Model for Understanding Graphical Perception , 1993, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[49]  R. Tourangeau,et al.  Fast times and easy questions: the effects of age, experience and question complexity on web survey response times , 2008 .

[50]  Antonio Maldonado,et al.  Improving risk understanding across ability levels: Encouraging active processing with dynamic icon arrays. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[51]  W. Gaissmaier,et al.  Numbers can be worth a thousand pictures: individual differences in understanding graphical and numerical representations of health-related information. , 2012, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.