Event-related potentials during an emotional Stroop task.

Emotional Stroop tasks have gained wide interest in scientific literature in the last two decades. Although no direct measure of attention is employed, these studies infer the presence of preferential processing of threatening information based on reaction time (RT) impairment in a competing task. Because event-related potential (ERP) measures are sensitive to both the extent (amplitude) and speed (latency) of cerebral processing, they are valuable tools with which to examine more directly the claim that threatening stimuli are associated with enhanced attention. Twenty-two students rated a pool of words to identify those that were personally disturbing. Two word types (threat and neutral) were then compared in two tasks (color relevant, in which the color ink of words was identified, and word relevant in which words were classified as threatening or not). No emotional Stroop effect was observed in terms of longer RTs to identify the colors of threat words. ERP results provided valuable information about threat processing which was not observed with behavioral measures. Threat content was associated with larger P2 amplitude in the right than left hemisphere, and larger P3 amplitude, across tasks. The results indicate strong evidence for enhanced processing of threat-related stimuli in healthy individuals. It is concluded that ERPs are a sensitive measure of processes underlying emotional Stroop performance, which can be used to elucidate attentional biases in healthy and clinical populations.

[1]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[2]  M. R. Kristian,et al.  Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers' treatment outcome. , 2002, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[3]  L Carretié,et al.  Emotion, attention, and the 'negativity bias', studied through event-related potentials. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[4]  Yue-Jia Luo,et al.  Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: An ERP study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  K. Mogg,et al.  A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[6]  R. McNally,et al.  Cognitive processing of idiographic emotional information in panic disorder. , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[7]  E. Bernat,et al.  Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[8]  H. Gray,et al.  P300 as an index of attention to self-relevant stimuli , 2004 .

[9]  Thomas F Münte,et al.  Temporal dynamics of early perceptual processing , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[10]  John Polich,et al.  P300 and response time from a manual Stroop task , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[11]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  J. Lannou,et al.  The Stroop's test evokes a negative brain potential, the N400. , 1997, The International journal of neuroscience.

[13]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[14]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Lateralization in Emotion and Emotional Disorders , 1998 .

[15]  J. Brosschot,et al.  The emotional Stroop interference effect in anxiety: attentional bias or cognitive avoidance? , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[16]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[17]  E. Sanavio,et al.  Compulsive Checking and Selective Processing of Threatening Information , 2001, Psychological reports.

[18]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .

[19]  P. van Oppen,et al.  Selective processing of emotional information in obsessive compulsive disorder. , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[20]  Chris R. Brewin,et al.  Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders , 1989 .

[21]  B. Dunn Computational Analysis , 2007 .

[22]  A. Weinstein Visual ERPs evidence for enhanced processing of threatening information in anxious university students , 1995, Biological Psychiatry.

[23]  T W Picton,et al.  The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[24]  H. McNeely,et al.  ERP indices of emotionality and semantic cohesiveness during recognition judgments. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[25]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Electrical neuroimaging reveals early generator modulation to emotional words , 2004, NeuroImage.

[26]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  S. Orr,et al.  Seeking the source of emotional Stroop interference effects in PTSD: A study of P3s to traumatic words , 1997, Integrative physiological and behavioral science : the official journal of the Pavlovian Society.

[28]  ● Pytorch,et al.  Attention! , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  The P300 component of the event-related brain potential as an index of information processing , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[30]  Edna B. Foa,et al.  Information processing in obsessive—compulsive disorder , 1993, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[31]  N. Fox,et al.  Individual differences in children’s performance during an emotional Stroop task: A behavioral and electrophysiological study , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[32]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Regional brain activity when selecting a response despite interference: An H2 15O PET study of the stroop and an emotional stroop , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[33]  D. Clark,et al.  Does anxiety lead to selective processing of threat-related information? , 1991, Behaviour research and therapy.

[34]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[35]  C MacLeod,et al.  Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimulus materials. , 1992, Behaviour research and therapy.

[36]  C. Nelson,et al.  Event-related potentials to emotional and neutral stimuli. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[37]  M. Bradley,et al.  Fleeting images: a new look at early emotion discrimination. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[38]  B. Kopell,et al.  The Stroop effect: brain potentials localize the source of interference. , 1981, Science.

[39]  O. John,et al.  Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  H. Mayberg,et al.  An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  R. Johnson,et al.  On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[42]  V. Johnston,et al.  Multiple P3s to emotional stimuli and their theoretical significance. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[43]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  Training and Stroop-like interference: evidence for a continuum of automaticity. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  R. West,et al.  Effects of task context and fluctuations of attention on neural activity supporting performance of the Stroop task , 2000, Brain Research.

[45]  S. Mineka,et al.  Hierarchical models of emotional disorders and emotion-congruent cognitive biases , 2004 .

[46]  M. Kutas,et al.  Electrophysiology of cognitive processing. , 1983, Annual review of psychology.

[47]  A. Beck,et al.  An information processing model of anxiety: automatic and strategic processes. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[48]  Francisco Mercado,et al.  Emotion and Attention Interaction Studied through Event-Related Potentials , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. , 1985, Behaviour research and therapy.

[50]  M. Kyrios,et al.  Automatic and strategic processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder: attentional bias, cognitive avoidance or more complex phenomena? , 1998, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[51]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Mechanisms and models of se-lective attention , 1995 .

[52]  R. Ratcliff Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[53]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[54]  A. Mathews,et al.  A Cognitive Model of Selective Processing in Anxiety , 1998, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[55]  G. Woodman,et al.  Event-related potential studies of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  R. West,et al.  Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  K. Mogg,et al.  Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[58]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[59]  K. Crowley,et al.  A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[60]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[61]  A. Angrilli,et al.  Visual evoked potentials, heart rate responses and memory to emotional pictorial stimuli. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[62]  L. Derogatis,et al.  The Brief Symptom Inventory: an introductory report , 1983, Psychological Medicine.

[63]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[64]  Dinkar Sharma,et al.  Intrusive Cognitions: An Investigation of the Emotional Stroop Task , 1995 .

[65]  C Alain,et al.  Event-related neural activity associated with the Stroop task. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[66]  K. Mogg,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states: a replication. , 1989, Behaviour research and therapy.

[67]  John E. Roberts,et al.  Memory and attention in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: a review. , 2005, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[68]  A. Pickering,et al.  Attentional bias in obsessional compulsive disorder. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[69]  W. Roth,et al.  The emotional Stroop effect in anxiety disorders: general emotional or disorder specificity? , 2001, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[70]  A. Wells,et al.  Modelling cognition in emotional disorder: the S-REF model. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[71]  P. Salkovskis,et al.  Information processing in spider phobics: the Stroop colour naming task may indicate strategic but not automatic attentional bias. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[72]  F. Michèl,et al.  Processing of Obsessive, Responsibility, Neutral Words and Pseudo-Words in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Study with Lexical Decision Test , 1995, Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy.

[73]  Mick Hunter,et al.  Visual Signal Detection Measured by Event-Related Potentials , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[74]  M. Verbraak,et al.  The emotional Stroop: a comparison of panic disorder patients, obsessive-compulsive patients, and normal controls, in two experiments. , 2002, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[75]  R. Snow,et al.  Review of the Evidence. , 1964, Science.