Movement trajectory smoothness is not associated with the endpoint accuracy of rapid multi-joint arm movements in young and older adults.

The minimum variance theory proposes that motor commands are corrupted by signal-dependent noise and smooth trajectories with low noise levels are selected to minimize endpoint error and endpoint variability. The purpose of the study was to determine the contribution of trajectory smoothness to the endpoint accuracy and endpoint variability of rapid multi-joint arm movements. Young and older adults performed arm movements (4 blocks of 25 trials) as fast and as accurately as possible to a target with the right (dominant) arm. Endpoint accuracy and endpoint variability along with trajectory smoothness and error were quantified for each block of trials. Endpoint error and endpoint variance were greater in older adults compared with young adults, but decreased at a similar rate with practice for the two age groups. The greater endpoint error and endpoint variance exhibited by older adults were primarily due to impairments in movement extent control and not movement direction control. The normalized jerk was similar for the two age groups, but was not strongly associated with endpoint error or endpoint variance for either group. However, endpoint variance was strongly associated with endpoint error for both the young and older adults. Finally, trajectory error was similar for both groups and was weakly associated with endpoint error for the older adults. The findings are not consistent with the predictions of the minimum variance theory, but support and extend previous observations that movement trajectories and endpoints are planned independently.

[1]  Evangelos A. Christou,et al.  Aging and Variability of Voluntary Contractions , 2011, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[2]  E. Englund,et al.  Neuronal loss in the brainstem and cerebellum--part of the normal aging process? A morphometric study of the vermis cerebelli and inferior olivary nucleus. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[3]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Altered aiming movements in Parkinson's disease patients and elderly adults as a function of delays in movement onset , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[5]  R. Miall,et al.  The curvature of human arm movements in the absence of visual experience , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  O. Lippold,et al.  Age-related impaired reflex sensitivity in a human hand muscle. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Time delays prior to movement alter the drawing kinematics of elderly adults. , 2003, Human movement science.

[9]  Yana Suchy,et al.  Age-related changes of the functional architecture of the cortico-basal ganglia circuitry during motor task execution , 2011, NeuroImage.

[10]  Paul L Gribble,et al.  Role of cocontraction in arm movement accuracy. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  G. Stelmach,et al.  The influence of mental and motor load on handwriting movements in parkinsonian patients. , 1998, Acta psychologica.

[13]  Stephen H. Scott,et al.  Hand and joint paths during reaching movements with and without vision , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  S. Scott,et al.  Random change in cortical load representation suggests distinct control of posture and movement , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  M. Sale,et al.  Age-related differences in corticospinal control during functional isometric contractions in left and right hands. , 2005, Journal of applied physiology.

[17]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  The role of execution noise in movement variability. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  S. Riek,et al.  Muscle coordination during rapid force production by young and older adults. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[20]  George E. Stelmach,et al.  Movement structure in young and elderly adults during goal-directed movements of the left and right arm , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  R. Enoka,et al.  Mechanisms that contribute to differences in motor performance between young and old adults. , 2003, Journal of electromyography and kinesiology : official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology.

[23]  J. Cooke,et al.  Control of simple arm movements in elderly humans , 1989, Neurobiology of Aging.

[24]  P. Morasso Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Elderly subjects are impaired in spatial coordination in fine motor control. , 1998, Acta psychologica.

[26]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[27]  A Eisen,et al.  Cortical projections to spinal motoneurons , 1996, Neurology.

[28]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  R. Sainburg Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Controlling the statistics of action: obstacle avoidance. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  R. Enoka,et al.  Coactivation of the antagonist muscle does not covary with steadiness in old adults. , 2000, Journal of applied physiology.

[32]  George E Stelmach,et al.  Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target size, and movement amplitude. , 2002, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[33]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Disruptions in the reach-to-grasp actions of Parkinson's patients , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Coactivation to reduce variability in the elderly. , 1998, Motor control.

[35]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Handedness: dominant arm advantages in control of limb dynamics. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Kelvin E. Jones,et al.  The scaling of motor noise with muscle strength and motor unit number in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  J. Cooke,et al.  Kinematics of arm movements in elderly humans , 1989, Neurobiology of Aging.

[38]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  D. Sternad,et al.  Decomposition of variability in the execution of goal-oriented tasks: three components of skill improvement. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Roger M Enoka,et al.  Different neural adjustments improve endpoint accuracy with practice in young and old adults. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  R. Enoka,et al.  Timing variability and not force variability predicts the endpoint accuracy of fast and slow isometric contractions , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  R. Enoka,et al.  Fluctuations in acceleration during voluntary contractions lead to greater impairment of movement accuracy in old adults. , 2003, Journal of applied physiology.

[43]  R. Carson,et al.  The consequences of resistance training for movement control in older adults. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[44]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis, 4th ed. , 2005 .

[45]  R. Enoka,et al.  Practice reduces motor unit discharge variability in a hand muscle and improves manual dexterity in old adults. , 2005, Journal of applied physiology.

[46]  L G Carlton,et al.  Old adults exhibit greater motor output variability than young adults only during rapid discrete isometric contractions. , 2001, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[47]  M. Kawato,et al.  Optimal impedance control for task achievement in the presence of signal-dependent noise. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .

[49]  W G Darling,et al.  A linked muscular activation model for movement generation and control. , 1987, Journal of Motor Behavior.

[50]  James Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  R. Enoka,et al.  Steadiness is reduced and motor unit discharge is more variable in old adults , 2000, Muscle & nerve.

[52]  Brian C. Clark,et al.  Older adults exhibit more intracortical inhibition and less intracortical facilitation than young adults , 2010, Experimental Gerontology.

[53]  R. Enoka,et al.  Fluctuations in motor output during steady contractions are weakly related across contraction types and between hands , 2005, Muscle & nerve.

[54]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Parkinsonism Reduces Coordination of Fingers, Wrist, and Arm in Fine Motor Control , 1997, Experimental Neurology.

[55]  T. Doherty Invited review: Aging and sarcopenia. , 2003, Journal of applied physiology.

[56]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Dissociation of initial trajectory and final position errors during visuomotor adaptation following unilateral stroke , 2009, Brain Research.

[57]  Ning Qian,et al.  An optimization principle for determining movement duration. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Julie Messier,et al.  Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  M. Kawato,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[60]  Claude Ghez,et al.  Separate adaptive mechanisms for controlling trajectory and final position in reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  Hank Heijink,et al.  Different learned coordinate frames for planning trajectories and final positions in reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  Naofumi Tanaka,et al.  Spinal excitation and inhibition decrease as humans age. , 2004, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[63]  J H Yan,et al.  Effects of Aging on Linear and Curvilinear Aiming Arm Movements , 2000, Experimental aging research.

[64]  Kelvin E. Jones,et al.  Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  Bernard E. Tomlinson,et al.  Cell counts in human cerebral cortex in normal adults throughout life using an image analysing computer , 1980, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[66]  R. Enoka,et al.  Practice and endpoint accuracy with the left and right hands of old adults: The right‐hemisphere aging model , 2008, Muscle & nerve.

[67]  R. Enoka,et al.  Endpoint accuracy for a small and a large hand muscle in young and old adults during rapid, goal-directed isometric contractions , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[68]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[69]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.