What Does Code Red Mean?

The U.S.Department of Homeland Security developed the Homeland Security Advisory System (HSAS)as a means to disseminate information regarding the risk of terrorist acts. HSAScommunicates a series of warnings in the form of fivegraduated threat conditions that utilize three distinct attributes: colors, words, and phrases. As the table below illustrates. each of these attributes is composed of five components that are ordered to communicate five distinct threat levels.The purpose of this additional study was to determine the effectiveness of HSASas a means of public hazard communication by empirically assessing people's understanding of the system. Our 57 participants (see page 7 in the accompanying article) were given a set of index card labels and asked to rank the colors, words. and phrases from most threatening to least threatening. During color label sorting. 57.9%of them erred by deviating from the correct order illustrated in the table below.When examining the specific errors made we found that the most frequent errors occurred when participants thought the blue threat condition was less threatening than the green condition (78%). Another source of error resulted from participants confusing the orange with the yellow threat condition (12.1%).Together. these twO categories of confusion account for more than 90% of the color-sorting errors. The word-sorting task revealed somewhat better results: only 33.3% of the sample deviated from the correct order. Participants most frequendy confused the guarded condition with the low condition (42.1%)and less frequendy confused the elevated with the guarded condition (21.1%).Together. these two categories of confusion account for more than 63% of the errors during word sorting. Participants were the most accurate when asked to order the descriptive phrases. compared with the other sorting tasks. In the phrase-sorting task. 26.3% deviated from the correct order. Participants most frequendy confused "Significant risk of terrorist attacks" with "High risk of terrorist attacks" (26.7%) and less frequently confused "Severe risk of terrorist attacks" with "High risk of terrorist attacks" (13.3%).Together. these categories of error accounted for 40% of the errors during phrase sorting. The error-laden results of the three sorting tasks provide converging evidence that HSASmay not be an effectivehazard communication tool. These results are not surprising when considered In the context of previous work within the human factors/ergonomics warnings literature (laughery. Wogalter. & Young,I994;Wogalter,Young,& Laughery,200 I). For instance, the results of the color-sorting task are consistent with previous research: Although the color red consistently connotes the presence of a hazard, orange and yellow are not readily differentiated on the hazard continuum (Chapanls, 1994).Colors such as red and yellow suggest greater levelsof hazard than other common colors, such as green and blue, but research indicates that people do not perceive blue to be mori! hazardous than green (Braun & Silver. 1995;Rashid & Wogalter, 1997).Thus, participants' poor performance on the color-sorting task was predictable. Had human factors/ergonomics professionals been consulted on the design of HSAS.the misunderstandings associated with color coding of threat levels might have been avoided, resulting In a more effective hazard communication tool. Similarconfusions in the wordand phrase-sorting tasks might also have been avoided in light of extensive research demonstrating that people frequently have difficultydistinguishing one signalword from another, such as "danger," "warning:' and "caution" (Braun.Kline,& Silver,1995;leonard. I999).The designers of HSASmight have avoided the use of this attribute entirely. At the very least, HSASshould have been tested prior to deployment to determine whether the public could distinguish among the five threat levels.Had this testing occurred and the findings refleCted the confusion revealed in the present results, alternative attributes such as numbers (I.e.. 1-5) or percentages (e.g.,80%) might have been explored as a means to disambiguate the threat levels.

[1]  Harm J. G. Zwaga,et al.  Visual Information For Everyday Use : Design And Research Perspectives , 1999 .

[2]  S. David Leonard Does color of warnings affect risk perception , 1999 .

[4]  Martin B. Curry,et al.  Exploring the effects of icon characteristics on user performance: the role of icon concreteness, complexity, and distinctiveness. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[5]  A. Chapanis Hazards associated with three signal words and four colours on warning signs , 1994 .

[6]  Michael S. Wogalter,et al.  Prohibitive pictorials:: Evaluations of different circle-slash negation symbols , 1998 .

[7]  Michael S. Wogalter,et al.  Comprehension and Memory , 1999 .

[8]  A. Fisk,et al.  Phrase Generation and Symbol Comprehension for 40 Safety Symbols , 2001 .

[9]  Curt C. Braun,et al.  The influence of color on warning label perceptions , 1995 .

[10]  Judy Edworthy,et al.  Warning design : a research prospective , 1996 .

[11]  Peter A. Hancock,et al.  Defeating Terrorism: What Can Human Factors/Ergonomics Offer? , 2002 .

[12]  Michael S. Wogalter,et al.  Applying Usability Engineering Principles to the Design and Testing of Warning Messages , 1999 .

[13]  N. C. Silver,et al.  Interaction of signal word and colour on warning labels: differences in perceived hazard and behavioural compliance. , 1995, Ergonomics.

[14]  Michael S. Wogalter,et al.  Comprehension of Pictorial Symbols: Effects of Context and Test Method , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[15]  S. Kumar Advances in Occupational Ergonomics and Safety , 1998 .

[16]  Michael S. Wogalter,et al.  Pharmaceutical container labels: enhancing preference perceptions with alternative designs and pictorials , 1996 .