The role of attentional bias in substance abuse.

There has been much recent interest in the idea that drug users show biased attention toward drug-related events. Because drug stimuli produce conditioned responses that may motivate drug taking, biased attention toward these cues may play an important role in drug use and relapse following treatment. The performance of drug users on the Stroop task and visual dot-probe task has been interpreted as demonstrating attentional bias toward drug cues specific to an individual's drug use history. However, studies often fail to include necessary control groups or comparison stimuli, thereby making it difficult to definitively conclude that reported results reflect a specific attentional response to personally relevant drug events. Although promising, these initial studies need to be followed up with better controlled demonstrations of attentional bias and with studies linking bias levels to other measures of drug taking.

[1]  K. Berridge,et al.  The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction , 1993, Brain Research Reviews.

[2]  Matt Field,et al.  Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. , 2003, Addiction.

[3]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Effect of psychological treatment on cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[4]  K. Mogg,et al.  A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[5]  J. Deakin,et al.  Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence , 2000, Psychological Medicine.

[6]  G. Yeates,et al.  Effects of alcohol cues on cognitive processing in heavy and light drinkers. , 1999, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[7]  B. Carter,et al.  Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. , 1999, Addiction.

[8]  Frank Ryan,et al.  Attentional bias and alcohol dependence: a controlled study using the modified stroop paradigm. , 2002, Addictive behaviors.

[9]  Karin Mogg,et al.  Covert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxiety , 2000 .

[10]  M. R. Kristian,et al.  Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers' treatment outcome. , 2002, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[11]  A. Childress,et al.  Role of conditioning factors in the development of drug dependence. , 1986, The Psychiatric clinics of North America.

[12]  T. Duka,et al.  Kindling of withdrawal: a study of craving and anxiety after multiple detoxifications in alcoholic inpatients. , 2002, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[13]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Discriminative stimuli that control instrumental tobacco-seeking by human smokers also command selective attention , 2003, Psychopharmacology.

[14]  M. Sayette,et al.  Effects of smoking opportunity on attentional bias in smokers. , 2001, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[15]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Alcoholic subjects' attentional bias in the processing of alcohol-related words. , 1994 .

[17]  A. Taylor,et al.  Psychological aspects of irritable bowel syndrome: comparisons with inflammatory bowel disease and nonpatient controls. , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.

[18]  Karin Mogg,et al.  Selective Processing of Smoking-Related Cues in Current Smokers, Ex-Smokers and Never-Smokers on the Modified Stroop Task , 2003, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[19]  S. Rachman,et al.  Part III — Intrusive thoughts: The effects of an uncontrived stress , 1981 .

[20]  H. de Wit,et al.  Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. , 1984, Psychological review.

[21]  J. Blount,et al.  Alcohol Abusers' and Nonabusers' Distraction by Alcohol and Concern-Related Stimuli , 2000, The American journal of drug and alcohol abuse.

[22]  W. Cox,et al.  The effects of alcohol cue exposure on non-dependent drinkers' attentional bias for alcohol-related stimuli. , 2003, Alcohol and alcoholism.

[23]  Anna Rose Childress,et al.  Conditioning factors in drug abuse: can they explain compulsion? , 1998, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[24]  C. Ray Examination Stress and Performance on a Color-Word Interference Test , 1979, Perceptual and motor skills.

[25]  T. Eissenberg,et al.  Effects of abstinence and smoking on information processing in adolescent smokers , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[26]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional and evaluative biases for smoking cues in nicotine dependence: component processes of biases in visual orienting , 2004, Behavioural pharmacology.

[27]  J F Thayer,et al.  Attentional bias in active smokers, abstinent smokers, and nonsmokers. , 1997, Addictive behaviors.

[28]  I. Franken,et al.  Influence of individual differences in craving and obsessive cocaine thoughts on attentional processes in cocaine abuse patients. , 2000, Addictive behaviors.

[29]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[30]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[31]  W. M. Cox,et al.  Alcohol-related words are distracting to both alcohol abusers and non-abusers in the Stroop colour-naming task. , 1998, Addiction.

[32]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Attentional bias to threat in clinical anxiety states , 1992 .

[33]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Anxiety and the Allocation of Attention to Threat , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[34]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  S. Mineka,et al.  Cognitive Biases and the Emotional Disorders , 1992 .

[36]  Karin Mogg,et al.  Selective processing of smoking-related cues in smokers: manipulation of deprivation level and comparison of three measures of processing bias , 2002, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[37]  T. Duka,et al.  Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers , 2001, Psychopharmacology.

[38]  S. Channon,et al.  The effect of short-term fasting on processing of food cues in normal subjects. , 1990 .

[39]  K. Mogg,et al.  Cognitive bias and drug craving in recreational cannabis users. , 2004, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[40]  S. Shiffman,et al.  Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. , 2003, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[41]  K. Dobson,et al.  Attentional biases in eating disorders: a meta-analytic review of Stroop performance. , 2004, Clinical psychology review.

[42]  H. Nordby,et al.  Alcoholics' selective attention to alcohol stimuli: automated processing? , 2000, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[43]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  J. Monterosso,et al.  Comparing attentional bias to smoking cues in current smokers, former smokers, and non-smokers using a dot-probe task. , 2002, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[45]  K. Mogg,et al.  Time course of attentional bias for threat information in non-clinical anxiety. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[46]  Helen Waters,et al.  A demonstration of attentional bias, using a novel dual task paradigm, towards clinically salient material in recovering alcohol abuse patients? , 2003, Psychological Medicine.

[47]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. , 1995, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[48]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: the roles of initial orienting and maintained attention , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[49]  P. Philippot,et al.  Selective attention to angry faces in clinical social phobia. , 2004, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[50]  Andrew J Waters,et al.  Attentional shifts to smoking cues in smokers. , 2003, Addiction.

[51]  C. Feyerabend,et al.  Determinants and effects of attentional bias in smokers. , 2000, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[52]  Lee Hogarth,et al.  Attentional orienting towards smoking-related stimuli , 2003, Behavioural pharmacology.

[53]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Selective processing of visual alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholics: an approach-avoidance conflict? , 1997, Addictive behaviors.

[54]  S. Tiffany,et al.  A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. , 1990, Psychological review.

[55]  F P McKenna,et al.  Colour naming of phobia-related words. , 1986, British journal of psychology.

[56]  Edna B. Foa,et al.  Information processing in obsessive—compulsive disorder , 1993, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[57]  J. Mattia,et al.  The revised Stroop color-naming task in social phobics. , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.

[58]  B. Jones,et al.  Social users of alcohol and cannabis who detect substance-related changes in a change blindness paradigm report higher levels of use than those detecting substance-neutral changes , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[59]  T. Duka,et al.  The priming effect of alcohol pre-load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[60]  Matt Field,et al.  Eye movements to smoking-related cues: effects of nicotine deprivation , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[61]  Matt Field,et al.  Attentional bias in drug dependence: vigilance for cigarette-related cues in smokers. , 2003, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[62]  I. Albery,et al.  Selective attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli in problem drinkers and non-problem drinkers. , 2001, Addiction.

[63]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. , 2004, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[64]  S Shiffman,et al.  A multi-dimensional analysis of cue-elicited craving in heavy smokers and tobacco chippers. , 2001, Addiction.

[65]  K. Mann,et al.  Effects of disease-related cues in alcoholic inpatients: results of a controlled "Alcohol Stroop" study. , 1995, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[66]  M. Jarvik,et al.  Nicotine abstinence produces content-specific stroop interference , 2005, Psychopharmacology.