Manual response preparation disrupts spatial attention: An electrophysiological investigation of links between action and attention

Previous behavioural and neuroscience studies have shown that the systems involved in the control of attention and action are functionally and anatomically linked. We used behavioural and event-related brain potential measures to investigate whether such links are mandatory or merely optional. Cues presented at the start of each trial instructed participants to shift attention to the left or right side and to simultaneously prepare to a finger movement with their left or right hand. In different trials, cues were followed by a central Go signal, requiring execution of the prepared manual response (motor task), or by a peripheral visual stimulus, which required a target-non-target discrimination only when presented on the cued side (attention task). Lateralised ERP components indicative of covert attention shifts were found when attention and action were directed to the same side (same side condition), but not when attention and action were directed to opposite sides (opposite sides condition). Likewise, effects of spatial attention on the processing of peripheral visual stimuli were present only when attention and action were directed to the same side, but not in the opposite sides condition. These results demonstrate that preparing a manual response on one side severely disrupts the attentional selection of visual stimuli on the other side, and suggest that it is not possible to simultaneously direct attention and action to different locations in space. They support the hypothesis that the control of spatial attention and action are implemented by shared brain circuits, and are therefore linked in a mandatory fashion.

[1]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  Frontoparietal control of spatial attention and motor intention in human EEG. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  R Verleger,et al.  Lateralized human cortical activity for shifting visuospatial attention and initiating saccades. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  M. Eimer “Sensory gating” as a mechanism for visuospatial orienting: Electrophysiological evidence from trial-by-trial cuing experiments , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Glyn W Humphreys,et al.  Interactions between perception and action programming: Evidence from visual extinction and optic ataxia , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[5]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Attention in Action: Advances from Cognitive Neuroscience , 1984 .

[6]  D. Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. II. Contributions of parietal cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  H. Deubel,et al.  Selective Dorsal and Ventral Processing: Evidence for a Common Attentional Mechanism in Reaching and Perception , 1998 .

[8]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Lateralized EEG components with direction information for the preparation of saccades versus finger movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Michael S. Beauchamp,et al.  A Parametric fMRI Study of Overt and Covert Shifts of Visuospatial Attention , 2001, NeuroImage.

[10]  D. Robinson,et al.  Parietal association cortex in the primate: sensory mechanisms and behavioral modulations. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Neurons of area 7 activated by both visual stimuli and oculomotor behavior , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  H. Deubel,et al.  Delayed Saccades, but Not Delayed Manual Aiming Movements, Require Visual Attention Shifts , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[14]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  An ERP study of preparatory and inhibitory mechanisms in a cued saccade task , 2006, Brain Research.

[15]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Visuomotor neurons: ambiguity of the discharge or 'motor' perception? , 2000, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[16]  S. Kobayashi,et al.  Electroencephalographic activity associated with shifts of visuospatial attention. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  Martin Eimer,et al.  ERP correlates of shared control mechanisms involved in saccade preparation and in covert attention , 2007, Brain Research.

[18]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  R Verleger,et al.  Lateralised cortical activity due to preparation of saccades and finger movements: a comparative study. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[20]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Manual response preparation and saccade programming are linked to attention shifts: ERP evidence for covert attentional orienting and spatially specific modulations of visual processing , 2006, Brain Research.

[21]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Organization of the Frontal Motor Cortex. , 2000, News in physiological sciences : an international journal of physiology produced jointly by the International Union of Physiological Sciences and the American Physiological Society.

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits for Spatial Attention and Unilateral Neglect , 1987 .

[23]  R. Eason Visual evoked potential correlates of early neural filtering during selective attention , 1981 .

[24]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[25]  Martin Eimer,et al.  The instructed context of a motor task modulates covert response preparation and shifts of spatial attention. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[26]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Neural Processes Involved in Directing Attention , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  A. Sterr,et al.  EEG dipole analysis of motor-priming foreperiod activity reveals separate sources for motor and spatial attention components , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[28]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  T. Moore,et al.  Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Eric Castet,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[31]  A. Nobre,et al.  The dynamics of shifting visuospatial attention revealed by event-related potentials , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  G. R Mangun,et al.  Shifting visual attention in space: an electrophysiological analysis using high spatial resolution mapping , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[33]  C. Connor,et al.  Covert attention suppresses neuronal responses in area 7a of the posterior parietal cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Attentional selection in sequential manual movements, movements around an obstacle and in grasping , 2005 .

[36]  Emilio Bizzi,et al.  Discharge of frontal eye field neurons during saccadic and following eye movements in unanesthetized monkeys , 1968, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  M. Eimer,et al.  Covert manual response preparation triggers attentional modulations of visual but not auditory processing , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[38]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Covert manual response preparation triggers attentional shifts: ERP evidence for the premotor theory of attention , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Sustained interactions between perception and action in visual extinction and neglect: Evidence from sequential pointing , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Functional Organization of Human Intraparietal and Frontal Cortex for Attending, Looking, and Pointing , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  F. Perrin,et al.  Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[42]  J. Findlay,et al.  The Relationship between Eye Movements and Spatial Attention , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[43]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Can attention be directed to opposite locations in different modalities? An ERP study , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[44]  M. Jeannerod Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. , 1987 .

[45]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Space and selective attention , 1994 .

[46]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[47]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  G. Luppino,et al.  Parietofrontal Circuits for Action and Space Perception in the Macaque Monkey , 2001, NeuroImage.

[49]  T Moore,et al.  Control of eye movements and spatial attention. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[50]  D. Robinson,et al.  Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  J. Bullier,et al.  Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streams , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[52]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[53]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Visual, presaccadic, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey lateral intraparietal area. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Attentional Selection of Multiple Goal Positions Before Rapid Hand Movement Sequences: An Event-related Potential Study , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  S. Zeki,et al.  The neurology of saccades and covert shifts in spatial attention: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[57]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Dissociating effector and movement direction selection during the preparation of manual reaching movements: Evidence from lateralized ERP components , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[58]  R. Verleger,et al.  Spatiotemporal overlap between brain activation related to saccade preparation and attentional orienting , 2006, Brain Research.

[59]  G. Rizzolatti Mechanisms of Selective Attention in Mammals , 1983 .

[60]  Gerald Westheimer,et al.  Grouping of contextual elements that affect vernier thresholds. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[61]  M Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[62]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Eye movements evoked by stimulation of frontal eye fields. , 1969, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Covert Visual Spatial Orienting and Saccades: Overlapping Neural Systems , 2000, NeuroImage.