“Ugh! That's disgusting!”: Identification of the characteristics of foods underlying rejections based on disgust

Previous research has demonstrated that individuals' beliefs about the disgusting properties of foods play a central role in predicting willingness to eat novel foods of either animal or non-animal origin (Martins & Pliner, in press). The present study aimed to identify what characteristics of foods make individuals perceive them as disgusting. In this study, participants read a set of scenarios designed to depict potentially disgusting foods; participants in Sample 1 rated the perceived disgustingness of the foods while participants in Sample 2 rated the foods on a variety of attributes relevant to theoretical conceptions of disgust. Multidimensional scaling revealed two dimensions, aversive textural properties of the foods and reminders of livingness/animalness, that accounted for most of the variability in ratings of perceived disgustingness of the foods depicted in the scenarios. Implications for our current conceptualization of disgust are examined.

[1]  C D Wilson,et al.  Consumer acceptance of vegetarian sweet potato products intended for space missions. , 1998, Life support & biosphere science : international journal of earth space.

[2]  M. Sterman,et al.  Nutritional modulation of neural function , 1988 .

[3]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Handbook of Emotions , 1993 .

[4]  L. Birch Effects of Peer Models' Food Choices and Eating Behaviors on Preschoolers' Food Preferences. , 1980 .

[5]  PM Kenyon,et al.  Attitudes Towards Meat-eating in Vegetarian and Non-vegetarian Teenage Girls in England—an Ethnographic Approach , 1998, Appetite.

[6]  R. Frank,et al.  The Contribution of Psychological and Sensory Factors to Food Preference Patterns as Measured by the Food Attitudes Survey (FAS) , 1995, Appetite.

[7]  M. Krondl,et al.  Social and biocultural determinants of food selection. , 1986, Progress in food & nutrition science.

[8]  A. Fallon,et al.  A perspective on disgust. , 1987, Psychological review.

[9]  M. Pelchat,et al.  Neophobia in humans and the special status of foods of animal origin , 1991, Appetite.

[10]  A. Fallon,et al.  The psychological bases of food rejections by humans , 1983 .

[11]  R. L. Babbitt,et al.  Antecedent Manipulation in the Treatment of Primary Solid Food Refusal , 1993, Behavior modification.

[12]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .

[13]  Eric H. Lenneberg,et al.  New directions in the study of language , 1964 .

[14]  Ø. Ueland,et al.  Attitudes towards meat and meat-eating among adolescents in Norway: a qualitative study , 2002, Appetite.

[15]  M. Pelchat,et al.  Reduction of Neophobia in Humans by Exposure to Novel Foods , 1993, Appetite.

[16]  E. Leach,et al.  Anthropological Aspects of Language: Animal Categories and Verbal Abuse , 1989 .

[17]  Marcia Levin Pelchat,et al.  “Try it. You'll like it”. Effects of information on willingness to try novel foods , 1995, Appetite.

[18]  D. Booth,et al.  Influences on Meat Avoidance Among British Students , 1996, Appetite.

[19]  P. Pliner The Effects of Mere Exposure on Liking for Edible Substances , 1982, Appetite.

[20]  S. Tambiah Animals Are Good to Think and Good to Prohibit , 1969 .

[21]  D. Booth Sensory influences on food intake. , 2009, Nutrition reviews.

[22]  P. Pliner,et al.  Effects of a Model on Food Neophobia in Humans , 1995, Appetite.

[23]  D. Coe,et al.  Use of texture fading in the treatment of food selectivity. , 1998, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[24]  P. Pliner,et al.  Human food choices: An examination of the factors underlying acceptance/rejection of novel and familiar animal and nonanimal foods , 2005, Appetite.

[25]  P. Rozin Cultural Approaches to Human Food Preferences , 1988 .

[26]  R. Downey,et al.  Reasons for eating: An exploratory cognitive analysis , 1992 .

[27]  M. Chinnan,et al.  Consumer acceptance of fresh-cut iceberg lettuce treated with 2% hydrogen peroxide and mild heat. , 2002, Journal of food protection.

[28]  A. Fallon,et al.  The psychological categorization of foods and non-foods: A preliminary taxonomy of food rejections , 1980, Appetite.

[29]  A. Angyal,et al.  Disgust and related aversions. , 1941 .

[30]  C. Izard The psychology of emotions , 1991 .

[31]  P. Pliner,et al.  Increasing Willingness to Taste Novel Foods: Effects of Nutrition and Taste Information , 1997, Appetite.

[32]  L. Dubé,et al.  Similarities and Differences in Affective and Cognitive Origins of Food Likings and Dislikes , 1997, Appetite.

[33]  W. Ahearn,et al.  An Assessment of Food Acceptance in Children with Autism or Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified , 2001, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[34]  L. Birch,et al.  What kind of exposure reduces children's food neophobia? Looking vs. tasting , 1987, Appetite.

[35]  L. Birch,et al.  I don't like it; I never tried it: Effects of exposure on two-year-old children's food preferences , 1982, Appetite.

[36]  M. Pelchat,et al.  “Try it; it's good and it's good for you ”: Effects of Taste and Nutrition Information on Willingness to Try Novel Foods , 1997, Appetite.