Spinning in the scanner: neural correlates of virtual reorientation.

Recent studies have used spatial reorientation task paradigms to identify underlying cognitive mechanisms of navigation in children, adults, and a range of animal species. Despite broad interest in this task across disciplines, little is known about the brain bases of reorientation. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine neural activity in adults during a virtual reality version of the reorientation task. Three environments that varied in the cues provided were studied: a rectangular room with 4 identical gray walls (Geometry), a square room with 3 gray walls and 1 red wall (Feature), and a rectangular room with 3 gray walls and 1 red wall (Feature + Geometry). Multiple areas within the medial temporal lobe (MTL) showed increased activation when a feature was present compared with when reorientation was based only on geometric cues. In contrast, reliance on geometric cues significantly activated a number of non-MTL structures, including the prefrontal cortex and inferior temporal gyrus. These results provide neural evidence for processing differences between the 2 types of cue as well as insight into developmental and comparative aspects of reorientation.

[1]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Whither geometry? Troubles of the geometric module , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  S. Shettleworth,et al.  Learning about Environmental Geometry: an Associative Model , 2022 .

[3]  Russell A. Epstein The cortical basis of visual scene processing , 2005 .

[4]  N. Burgess,et al.  Geometric determinants of human spatial memory , 2004, Cognition.

[5]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual-Task Studies of Space and Language , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  J. Spencer,et al.  The Emerging Spatial Mind , 2007 .

[7]  Craig E. L. Stark,et al.  When zero is not zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Shape parameters explain data from spatial transformations: comment on Pearce et al. (2004) and Tommasi & Polli (2004). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[11]  Nora S Newcombe,et al.  Memory binding in early childhood: evidence for a retrieval deficit. , 2009, Child development.

[12]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Volumetry of hippocampus and amygdala with high-resolution MRI and three-dimensional analysis software: minimizing the discrepancies between laboratories. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[14]  Luca Tommasi,et al.  Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[15]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  THE DEVELOPMENT OF SPATIAL LOCATION CODING: PLACE LEARNING AND DEAD RECKONING IN THE SECOND AND THIRD YEARS , 1998 .

[16]  S. Thompson-Schill Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: inferring “how” from “where” , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  E. Spelke,et al.  Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.

[18]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.

[19]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[20]  M Zanforlin,et al.  Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). , 1990, Journal of comparative psychology.

[21]  Nora S Newcombe,et al.  Binding, relational memory, and recall of naturalistic events: a developmental perspective. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  Nora S Newcombe,et al.  Why size counts: children's spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures. , 2008, Developmental science.

[23]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Five Reasons to Doubt the Existence of a Geometric Module , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[24]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.

[25]  Peter L. Hurd,et al.  Growing in Circles , 2007 .

[26]  Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.  The shape of human navigation: How environmental geometry is used in maintenance of spatial orientation , 2008, Cognition.

[27]  Allen Cheung,et al.  The information content of panoramic images I: The rotational errors and the similarity of views in rectangular experimental arenas. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[28]  Peter M. Jones,et al.  Transfer of spatial behavior between different environments: implications for theories of spatial learning and for the role of the hippocampus in spatial learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[29]  N. Burgess Spatial Cognition and the Brain , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Knowing Where Things Are in the Second Year of Life: Implications for Hippocampal Development , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Explaining the Development of Spatial Reorientation , 2007 .

[33]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[34]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cognitive Strategies Dependent on the Hippocampus and Caudate Nucleus in Human Navigation: Variability and Change with Practice , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Charan Ranganath,et al.  Medial Temporal Lobe Activity Predicts Successful Relational Memory Binding , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Is language necessary for human spatial reorientation? Reconsidering evidence from dual task paradigms , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[37]  Laurie L Bloomfield,et al.  Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: features overshadow geometry , 2005, Biology Letters.

[38]  Stefan Köhler,et al.  Differential contributions of the parahippocampal place area and the anterior hippocampus to human memory for scenes , 2002, Hippocampus.

[39]  Jennifer E. Sutton,et al.  What is geometric information and how do animals use it? , 2009, Behavioural Processes.

[40]  P. Matthews,et al.  Semantic Processing in the Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex: A Combined Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[42]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[43]  Russell A. Epstein Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  Matthew R. Johnson Spatial Cognition, Spatial Perception , 2011, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[45]  Debbie M. Kelly,et al.  Pigeons' (Columba livia) encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. , 1998 .

[46]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Volumetry of temporopolar, perirhinal, entorhinal and parahippocampal cortex from high-resolution MR images: considering the variability of the collateral sulcus. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  Robert S. Astur,et al.  Factors affecting the hippocampal BOLD response during spatial memory , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[48]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system , 1993 .

[49]  Peter M. Jones,et al.  Further evidence that rats rely on local rather than global spatial information to locate a hidden goal: reply to Cheng and Gallistel (2005). , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[50]  Ingrid R. Olson,et al.  Working Memory for Conjunctions Relies on the Medial Temporal Lobe , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.