Overlapping features can be parsed on the basis of rapid temporal cues that produce stable emergent percepts

When two overlapping displays alternate rapidly, it is difficult to resolve the temporal coincidence of objects, parts, or features. However, under certain conditions (at least for luminance-based stimuli) rapid temporal coincidence can be detected on the basis of stable emergent percepts in which parts that oscillate in phase appear more strongly grouped than (or appear distinct from) parts that oscillate out of phase. These emergent percepts appear as depth segregation, enhanced slow orientation rivalry, and oriented shimmer (a new phenomenon that cannot be explained in terms of conventional apparent motion or temporal contrast illusions). These percepts resulted in up to an eightfold decrease in the coincidence detection threshold (alternations as fast as 20 ms/frame or 25 Hz) relative to control conditions that did not yield them; these sensitivity enhancements are unlikely to be due to temporal probability summation. The results provide psychophysical evidence that temporal-phase information can contribute to the parsing of overlapping patterns.

[1]  J. Movshon,et al.  Cortical oscillatory responses do not affect visual segmentation , 1996, Vision Research.

[2]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Binocular Rivalry and Motion Perception , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  R. Blake,et al.  Rival ideas about binocular rivalry , 1999, Vision Research.

[4]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Limits of attentive tracking reveal temporal properties of attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[5]  C. Gray The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration Still Alive and Well , 1999, Neuron.

[6]  Noboru Sugie,et al.  Neural models of brightness perception and retinal rivalry in binocular vision , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[7]  D. Sagi,et al.  Configuration saliency revealed in short duration binocular rivalry , 1999, Vision Research.

[8]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[9]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the Macaque. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  D. Ts'o,et al.  The organization of chromatic and spatial interactions in the primate striate cortex , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Interactions between global motion and local binocular rivalry , 1998, Vision Research.

[12]  R. Desimone,et al.  Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Improvement in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: Parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys , 1995, Neuron.

[14]  R. Blake,et al.  What is Suppressed during Binocular Rivalry? , 1980, Perception.

[15]  J. Ross,et al.  Spatial limitations of temporal segmentation , 1999, Vision Research.

[16]  G. Orban,et al.  Activity of inferior temporal neurons during orientation discrimination with successively presented gratings. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Neuronal synchrony does not represent texture segregation , 1998, Nature.

[18]  Mark A. Georgeson,et al.  Eye movements, afterimages and monocular rivalry , 1984, Vision Research.

[19]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Visual competition , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  Michael A. Webster,et al.  Temporal properties of brightness and color induction , 1986, Vision Research.

[21]  Marcia Grabowecky,et al.  Evidence for Perceptual “Trapping” and Adaptation in Multistable Binocular Rivalry , 2002, Neuron.

[22]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Luminance and chromatic modulation sensitivity of macaque ganglion cells and human observers. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[23]  E. Niebur,et al.  Modeling the Temporal Dynamics of IT Neurons in Visual Search: A Mechanism for Top-Down Selective Attention , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  S. Anstis,et al.  Monocular lustre from flicker , 2000, Vision Research.

[25]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry , 1996, Nature.

[26]  Arthur Bradley,et al.  The role of eye movements and masking in monocular rivalry , 1988, Vision Research.

[27]  David A. Leopold,et al.  What is rivalling during binocular rivalry? , 1996, Nature.

[28]  R. Blake,et al.  Binocular rivalry suppression: insensitive to spatial frequency and orientation change. , 1974, Vision research.

[29]  M. Usher,et al.  The effects of temporal synchrony on the perceived organization of elements in spatially symmetric and asymmetric grids , 2001 .

[30]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Visual object recognition. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  M Fahle,et al.  Figure–ground discrimination from temporal information , 1993, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[32]  Wolf Singer,et al.  Time as Coding Space in Neocortical Processing: A Hypothesis , 1994 .

[33]  F. Campbell,et al.  The Dependence of Monocular Rivalry on Spatial Frequency , 1973 .

[34]  UTE LEONARDS,et al.  The Influence of Temporal Phase Differences on Texture Segmentation , 1996, Vision Research.

[35]  F. Campbell,et al.  The Dependence of Monocular Rivalry on Orientation , 1973 .

[36]  Peter H. Schiller,et al.  The ON and OFF channels of the visual system , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[37]  A. Karni,et al.  A transition between eye and object rivalry determined by stimulus coherence , 2001, Vision Research.

[38]  D. Pollen,et al.  Spatial and temporal frequency selectivity of neurones in visual cortical areas V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  N J Wade,et al.  Monocular and Binocular Rivalry between Contours , 1975, Perception.

[40]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Synchrony Unbound A Critical Evaluation of the Temporal Binding Hypothesis , 1999, Neuron.

[41]  W. Singer,et al.  Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties , 1989, Nature.

[42]  W. Levelt On binocular rivalry , 1965 .

[43]  S. Magnussen,et al.  Effects of steady surround illumination on the brightness and darkness enhancement of flickering lights , 1975, Vision Research.

[44]  R. Blake A Neural Theory of Binocular Rivalry , 1989 .

[45]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of macaque V2. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  R. Shapley,et al.  Temporal-frequency selectivity in monkey visual cortex , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[47]  S. Magnussen,et al.  Temporal frequency characteristics of spatial interaction in human vision , 1975, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  Campbell Fw,et al.  Monocular alternation: a method for the investigation of pattern vision. , 1972 .

[49]  S. R. Lehky An Astable Multivibrator Model of Binocular Rivalry , 1988, Perception.

[50]  Michael A. Paradiso,et al.  The Representation of Brightness in Primary Visual Cortex , 1996, Science.

[51]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.

[52]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .

[53]  R. Desimone,et al.  Inferior temporal mechanisms for invariant object recognition. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  D. Heeger,et al.  Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  B. B. Lee,et al.  Amplitude and phase of responses of macaque retinal ganglion cells to flickering stimuli. , 1989, The Journal of physiology.

[56]  W. Singer,et al.  Two segmentation mechanisms with differential sensitivity for colour and luminance contrast , 1998, Vision Research.

[57]  L. Lack Selective attention and the control of binocular rivalry , 1978 .

[58]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organization. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[59]  B. B. Lee,et al.  The physiological basis of heterochromatic flicker photometry demonstrated in the ganglion cells of the macaque retina. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[60]  I. Kovács,et al.  When the brain changes its mind: interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  Marius Usher,et al.  Visual synchrony affects binding and segmentation in perception , 1998, Nature.

[62]  Minami Ito,et al.  Size and position invariance of neuronal responses in monkey inferotemporal cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Spatiotemporal organization of simple-cell receptive fields in the cat's striate cortex. II. Linearity of temporal and spatial summation. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  R Vogels,et al.  Responses of monkey inferior temporal neurons to luminance-, motion-, and texture-defined gratings. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[66]  Diane C. Rogers-Ramachandran,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for boundary and surface systems in human vision , 1998, Vision Research.

[67]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[68]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[69]  Peter H. Schiller,et al.  The connections of the retinal on and off pathways to the lateral geniculate nucleus of the monkey , 1984, Vision Research.

[70]  C. Gilbert Horizontal integration and cortical dynamics , 1992, Neuron.

[71]  S. Anstis,et al.  Nonlinear combination of luminance excursions during flicker, simultaneous contrast, afterimages and binocular fusion , 1998, Vision Research.

[72]  W. Singer,et al.  Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[73]  R. Blake,et al.  Do recognizable figures enjoy an advantage in binocular rivalry? , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[74]  A. Sanders Attention and performance III : proceedings of a symposium on attention and performance, held in Soesterberg August 4-8,1969, under the auspices of the Institute for Perception RVO-TNO, Soesterberg, the Netherlands , 1970 .

[75]  N. Logothetis,et al.  What is rivalling during binocular rivalry , 1996 .

[76]  A. Gorea,et al.  Perceptual bistability with counterphase gratings , 1984, Vision Research.

[77]  N. Logothetis Single units and conscious vision. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[78]  E. Miller,et al.  Suppression of visual responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex of the awake macaque by addition of a second stimulus , 1993, Brain Research.

[79]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[80]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[81]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Early binding of feature pairs for visual perception , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.