Cross-notation number priming investigated at different stimulus onset asynchronies in parity and naming tasks.

In this paper, a parity judgment task and a number naming task were used to investigate cross-notational number priming. Primes and targets could be verbal (e.g., seven) or Arabic numbers (e.g., 7), and were always presented in a different notation within the same trial (either a verbal prime and an Arabic target or an Arabic prime and a verbal target). Previous experiments showed that response latencies increase when the distance between prime and target increases (for example, in a naming task, seven is pronounced faster after 6 than after 5). This semantic distance priming effect was the same for Arabic and verbal targets and was the same for within-notation trials as for cross-notation trials. In the present experiments, we wanted to investigate whether the cross-notational priming effect also occurs at SOAs shorter than the ones used in previous experiments. Therefore, we used SOAs of 43, 57, 86, and 115 ms. Semantic distance effects were indeed present at these shorter SOAs: Processing times in the semantic parity judgment task and in the non-semantic naming task increased when the distance between prime and target increased. The results are discussed and integrated within an interactive dual-route model of number processing that postulates that the impact of the semantic and the non-semantic route depends on the task and the notation of the stimuli.

[1]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Three Cognitive Markers of Unconscious Semantic Activation , 1996, Science.

[2]  A. Marcel Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Automatic stimulus-response associations may be semantically mediated , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[4]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  W Fias,et al.  Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[7]  B. Butterworth,et al.  Toward a multiroute model of number processing: Impaired number transcoding with preserved calculation skills. , 1995 .

[8]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Semantic priming in number naming , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[9]  S Dehaene,et al.  Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[11]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[12]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious Masked Priming Depends on Temporal Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[13]  Patricia A. McMullen,et al.  Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia , 1999 .

[14]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Are Arabic numerals processed as pictures in a Stroop interference task? , 2001, Psychological research.

[15]  K. Forster,et al.  REPETITION PRIMING AND FREQUENCY ATTENUATION IN LEXICAL ACCESS , 1984 .

[16]  G. Underwood Strategies of information processing , 1980 .

[17]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task , 2001, NeuroImage.

[18]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  The issue of semantic mediation in word and number naming , 2006 .

[19]  P. F. Jong Understanding Normal and Impaired Reading Development: A Working Memory Perspective , 2006 .

[20]  S Dehaene,et al.  Number words and number non-words. A case of deep dyslexia extending to Arabic numerals. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  M. Coltheart Lexical access in simple reading tasks , 1978 .

[22]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Basic processes in reading : visual word recognition , 1993 .

[23]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[24]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[25]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  W. Fias The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC Effect , 1996 .

[27]  M. Damian,et al.  Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: automaticity rather than semantic processing. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.

[29]  David C. Plaut,et al.  Semantic and Associative Priming in a Distributed Attractor Network , 1995 .

[30]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Primed numbers : Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming , 1999 .

[31]  Marinella Cappelletti,et al.  Spared numerical abilities in a case of semantic dementia , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  A. Columbus Advances in Psychology Research , 2005 .

[33]  K. Paap,et al.  Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race , 1991 .

[34]  W Fias,et al.  Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect , 2001, Psychological research.

[35]  Marinella Cappelletti,et al.  Category specificity in reading and writing: the case of number words , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  M. Brysbaert Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. , 1995 .

[37]  X. Seron,et al.  Lexicalization Errors in Writing Arabic Numerals - A Single-Case Study , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[38]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli , 2001, Cognition.

[39]  K. Briand,et al.  Priming single digit numbers: automatic spreading activation dissipates as a function of semantic distance , 1986 .

[40]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Differential Contributions of the Left and Right Inferior Parietal Lobules to Number Processing , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line , 1999, Cognition.

[42]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[43]  W. Fias,et al.  Semantic Influences on Feature-Based Attention Due to Overlap of Neural Circuits , 2002, Cortex.

[44]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[45]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Long-term semantic memory versus contextual memory in unconscious number processing. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.