Is the organisation of goal-directed action modality specific? A common temporal structure
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Decety,et al. Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a pet investigation. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[2] Peter T. Fox,et al. The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape , 1998 .
[3] A. Wohlschläger,et al. Mental and manual rotation , 1998 .
[4] E. Procyk,et al. Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[5] J Hermsdörfer,et al. Kinematic analysis of movement imitation in apraxia. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[6] A. Sirigu,et al. The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.
[7] G. Goldenberg. Defective imitation of gestures in patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[8] M. Jeannerod. Mental imagery in the motor context , 1995, Neuropsychologia.
[9] Michael I. Jordan,et al. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.
[10] L. Parsons,et al. Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.
[11] K M Heilman,et al. Joint coordination deficits in limb apraxia. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[12] J. Mazziotta,et al. Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography , 1994, Nature.
[13] L. Jakobson,et al. Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[14] K M Heilman,et al. Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[15] M Wiesendanger,et al. Temporal coordination in bimanual actions. , 1994, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.
[16] M. Jeannerod,et al. Impairment of grasping movements following a bilateral posterior parietal lesion , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[17] M. Wiesendanger,et al. Temporal Structure of a Bimanual Goal‐directed Movement Sequence in Monkeys , 1994, The European journal of neuroscience.
[18] M. A. Arbib,et al. Models of Trajectory Formation and Temporal Interaction of Reach and Grasp. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[19] Umberto Castiello,et al. The bilateral reach to grasp movement , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.
[20] C. MacKenzie,et al. The effects of object weight on the kinematics of prehension. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.
[21] M. Jeannerod,et al. The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.
[22] T. Shallice. From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .
[23] Christopher Kennard,et al. Recent Advances in Clinical Neurology , 1988 .
[24] M. Jeannerod,et al. Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.
[25] M. Jeannerod. The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.
[26] C. MacKenzie,et al. Bimanual Movement Control: Information processing and Interaction Effects , 1984 .
[27] E. Renzi,et al. Modality-specific and supramodal mechanisms of apraxia. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[28] D Goodman,et al. On the coordination of two-handed movements. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[30] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[31] R. Passingham,et al. Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.