Influence of head orientation on visually and memory-guided arm movements.

In the absence of visual supervision, tilting the head sideways gives rise to deviations in spatially defined arm movements. The purpose of this study was to determine whether these deviations are restricted to situations with impoverished visual information. Two experiments were conducted in which participants were positioned supine and reproduced with their unseen index finger a 2 dimensional figure either under visual supervision or from memory (eyes closed). In the former condition, the figure remained visible (using a mirror). In the latter condition, the figure was first observed and then reproduced from memory. Participants' head was either aligned with the trunk or tilted 30° towards the left or right shoulder. In experiment 1, participants observed first the figure with the head straight and then reproduced it with the head either aligned or tilted sideways. In Experiment 2, participants observed the figure with the head in the position in which the figure was later reproduced. Results of Experiment 1 and 2 showed deviations of the motor reproduction in the direction opposite to the head in both the memory and visually-guided conditions. However, the deviations decreased significantly under visual supervision when the head was tilted left. In Experiment 1, the perceptual visual bias induced by head tilt was evaluated. Participants were required to align the figure parallel to their median trunk axis. Results revealed that the figure was perceived as parallel with the trunk when it was actually tilted in the direction of the head. Perceptual and motor responses did not correlate. Therefore, as long as visual feedback of the arm is prevented, an internal bias, likely originating from head/trunk representation, alters hand-motor production irrespectively of whether visual feedback of the figure is available or not.

[1]  D E Parker,et al.  Spatial orientation: Visual-vestibular-somatic interaction , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Hermann Aubert,et al.  Eine scheinbare bedeutende Drehung von Objecten bei Neigung des Kopfes nach rechts oder links , 1861, Archiv für pathologische Anatomie und Physiologie und für klinische Medicin.

[4]  Dietmar Ott,et al.  Vestibular-neck interaction of human ocular counterroll , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[5]  J F Soechting,et al.  Moving in three-dimensional space: frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  C. Bard,et al.  Reference systems for coding spatial information in normal subjects and a deafferented patient , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 1986, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Complementary Cognitive and Motor Image Processing , 1991 .

[9]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space. , 2004, Motor control.

[10]  Joanna J. Knox,et al.  Changes in head and neck position affect elbow joint position sense , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Effects of a tilted visual background on human sensory-motor coordination , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  D. Elliott Continuous visual information may be important after all: a failure to replicate Thomson (1983) , 1986 .

[13]  I Kozlovskaya,et al.  The effect of head-to-trunk position on the direction of arm movements before, during, and after space flight. , 1998, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[14]  J Blouin,et al.  Galvanic vestibular stimulation in humans produces online arm movement deviations when reaching towards memorized visual targets , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[15]  M Guerraz,et al.  The role of neck afferents in subjective orientation in the visual and tactile sensory modalities. , 2000, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[16]  M. Guerraz,et al.  Head Orientation Involvement in Assessment of the Subjective Vertical during Whole Body Tilt , 1998, Perceptual and motor skills.

[17]  A. Pouget,et al.  Multisensory spatial representations in eye-centered coordinates for reaching , 2002, Cognition.

[18]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  The Head-neck sensory motor system , 1992 .

[19]  M. Guerraz,et al.  The role of head-centric spatial reference with a static and kinetic visual disturbance , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  Fred H. Previc,et al.  The relationship between eye dominance and head tilt in humans , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  E A Roy,et al.  The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. , 2001, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[22]  Michel Guerraz,et al.  Reference frames and haptic perception of orientation: Body and head tilt effects on the oblique effect , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  J. J. Bloomberg,et al.  Context-dependent arm pointing adaptation , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  J. Paillard Brain and space , 1991 .

[25]  J. Paillard Motor and representational framing of space , 1991 .

[26]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  From head orientation to hand control: evidence of both neck and vestibular involvement in hand drawing , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  J. Soechting,et al.  Changes in spatial scale in drawing and handwriting: kinematic contributions by proximal and distal joints , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  Kathrin Finke,et al.  Effects of lateral head inclination on multimodal spatial orientation judgments in neglect: Evidence for impaired spatial orientation constancy , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  C. Hesse,et al.  Visual illusions, delayed grasping, and memory: No shift from dorsal to ventral control , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  G. Kerkhoff Multimodal spatial orientation deficits in left-sided visual neglect , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  W. Becker,et al.  Perception of angular head position during attempted alignment with eccentric visual objects , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  J. Crémieux,et al.  Perceived versus actual head-on-trunk orientation during arm movement control , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  J. Vercher,et al.  Internally driven control of reaching movements: A study on a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[34]  C. Hesse,et al.  Memory mechanisms in grasping , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  F. Previc,et al.  Direction and reliability of head tilt in humans. , 1996, Laterality.

[36]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[37]  Illusory changes in head position induced by neck muscle vibration can alter the perception of elbow position. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.

[38]  F H Previc,et al.  A general theory concerning the prenatal origins of cerebral lateralization in humans. , 1991, Psychological review.

[39]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[40]  K. E. Popov,et al.  Egocentric references and human spatial orientation in microgravity , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Lawrence Stark,et al.  Presbyopia research : from molecular biology to visual adaptation , 1991 .

[42]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  Internal representation of gaze direction with and without retinal inputs in man , 1995, Neuroscience Letters.

[43]  Peter Dixon,et al.  Motor adaptation to an optical illusion , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  William B. Templeton,et al.  The role of gravitational cues in the judgment of visual orientation , 1973 .

[45]  J. Vercher,et al.  Visual signals contribute to the coding of gaze direction , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  Luis F. Schettino,et al.  Effects of object shape and visual feedback on hand configuration during grasping , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Effects of an illusory orientation of the head on straight-ahead pointing movements , 1998 .

[48]  J Dichgans,et al.  Optokinetic-graviceptive interaction in different head positions. , 1974, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[49]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .