Competing Theories of Multialternative, Multiattribute Preferential Choice

In accounting for phenomena present in preferential choice experiments, modern models assume a wide array of different mechanisms such as lateral inhibition, leakage, loss aversion, and saliency. These mechanisms create interesting predictions for the dynamics of the deliberation process as well as the aggregate behavior of preferential choice in a variety of contexts. However, the models that embody these different mechanisms are rarely subjected to rigorous quantitative tests of suitability by way of model fitting and evaluation. Recently, complex, stochastic models have been cast aside in favor of simpler approximations, which may or may not capture the data as well. In this article, we use a recently developed method to fit the four extant models of context effects to data from two experiments: one involving consumer goods stimuli, and another involving perceptual stimuli. Our third study investigates the relative merits of the mechanisms currently assumed by the extant models of context effects by testing every possible configuration of mechanism within one overarching model. Across all tasks, our results emphasize the importance of several mechanisms such as lateral inhibition, loss aversion, and pairwise attribute differences, as these mechanisms contribute positively to model performance. Together, our results highlight the notion that mathematical tractability, while certainly a convenient feature of any model, should neither be the primary impetus for model development nor the promoting or demotion of specific model mechanisms. Instead, model fit, balanced with model complexity, should be the greatest burden to bear for any theoretical account of empirical phenomena.

[1]  Marius Usher,et al.  Extending a biologically inspired model of choice: multi-alternatives, nonlinearity and value-based multidimensional choice , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[2]  Andrew Howes,et al.  The Effect of Expected Value on Attraction Effect Preference Reversals , 2016, Journal of behavioral decision making.

[3]  A. Tversky,et al.  Context-dependent preferences , 1993 .

[4]  G. Schwarz Estimating the Dimension of a Model , 1978 .

[5]  Cajo J. F. ter Braak,et al.  A Markov Chain Monte Carlo version of the genetic algorithm Differential Evolution: easy Bayesian computing for real parameter spaces , 2006, Stat. Comput..

[6]  Christopher P. Puto,et al.  Adding Asymmetrically Dominated Alternatives: Violations of Regularity & the Similarity Hypothesis. , 1981 .

[7]  C. Robert,et al.  Deviance information criteria for missing data models , 2006 .

[8]  Jiyun Li,et al.  Theoretical developments in decision field theory: comment on Tsetsos, Usher, and Chater (2010). , 2010, Psychological review.

[9]  J. Busemeyer,et al.  A probabilistic, dynamic, and attribute-wise model of intertemporal choice. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  Sudeep Bhatia,et al.  The dynamics of deferred decision , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  William R. Holmes,et al.  A practical guide to the Probability Density Approximation (PDA) with improved implementation and error characterization , 2015 .

[12]  I. J. Myung,et al.  The Importance of Complexity in Model Selection. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[13]  Marius Usher,et al.  Using Time-Varying Evidence to Test Models of Decision Dynamics: Bounded Diffusion vs. the Leaky Competing Accumulator Model , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[14]  I. Simonson,et al.  Choice Based on Reasons: The Case of Attraction and Compromise Effects , 1989 .

[15]  W. Ma,et al.  Factorial comparison of working memory models. , 2014, Psychological review.

[16]  M. Humphreys,et al.  A context noise model of episodic word recognition. , 2001, Psychological review.

[17]  A. Rangel,et al.  Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Individual Choice Behavior: A Theoretical Analysis , 1979 .

[19]  I. J. Myung,et al.  Toward a method of selecting among computational models of cognition. , 2002, Psychological review.

[20]  J. Dana,et al.  Transitivity of preferences. , 2011, Psychological review.

[21]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  A ballistic model of choice response time. , 2005, Psychological review.

[23]  I. J. Myung,et al.  GUEST EDITORS' INTRODUCTION: Special Issue on Model Selection , 2000 .

[24]  Sudeep Bhatia Comparing Theories of Reference-Dependent Choice , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  R. Shepard,et al.  Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. , 1987, Science.

[26]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.

[27]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  Hierarchical Approximate Bayesian Computation , 2013, Psychometrika.

[28]  Gideon Nave,et al.  Extrapolative Beliefs in Perceptual and Economic Decisions: Evidence of a Common Mechanism , 2016, Manag. Sci..

[29]  On Amir,et al.  Choice Construction versus Preference Construction: The Instability of Preferences Learned in Context , 2007 .

[30]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[31]  D. Krantz Conjoint measurement: The Luce-Tukey axiomatization and some extensions ☆ , 1964 .

[32]  A. Parducci Category judgment: a range-frequency model. , 1965, Psychological review.

[33]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[35]  T. Ando Bayesian predictive information criterion for the evaluation of hierarchical Bayesian and empirical Bayes models , 2007 .

[36]  Jay I. Myung,et al.  Analytic Expressions for the BCDMEM Model of Recognition Memory. , 2007, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[37]  Marius Usher,et al.  Testing Multi-Alternative Decision Models with Non-Stationary Evidence , 2011, Front. Neurosci..

[38]  H. Simon Models of Bounded Rationality: Empirically Grounded Economic Reason , 1997 .

[39]  Takao Noguchi,et al.  In the attraction, compromise, and similarity effects, alternatives are repeatedly compared in pairs on single dimensions , 2014, Cognition.

[40]  Clintin P Davis-Stober,et al.  Reported violations of rationality may be aggregation artifacts , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  Journal of Mathematical Psychology a Tutorial on Approximate Bayesian Computation , 2022 .

[42]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Order effects in belief updating: The belief-adjustment model , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[43]  Ellen Peters,et al.  Assessing “Economic Value” , 2014, Psychological science.

[44]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The power law repealed: The case for an exponential law of practice , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[45]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  A generalized, likelihood-free method for posterior estimation , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[46]  I. J. Myung,et al.  Applying Occam’s razor in modeling cognition: A Bayesian approach , 1997 .

[47]  A. Tversky Features of Similarity , 1977 .

[48]  Shayne Loft,et al.  Slow down and remember to remember! A delay theory of prospective memory costs. , 2015, Psychological review.

[49]  John R. Anderson Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications , 1980 .

[50]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[51]  John W. Payne,et al.  Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: An information search and protocol analysis☆ , 1976 .

[52]  B. Love,et al.  The myth of computational level theory and the vacuity of rational analysis , 2011, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[53]  Rongjun Yu,et al.  The development of the asymmetrically dominated decoy effect in young children , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[54]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Building Bridges between Perceptual and Economic Decision-Making: Neural and Computational Mechanisms , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[55]  M. F. Luce,et al.  Constructive Consumer Choice Processes , 1998 .

[56]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  Approximate Bayesian computation with differential evolution , 2012 .

[57]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  The anchor integration model: A descriptive model of anchoring effects , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.

[58]  P. Slovic The Construction of Preference , 1995 .

[59]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Loss aversion and inhibition in dynamical models of multialternative choice. , 2004, Psychological review.

[60]  A. Tversky Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. , 1972 .

[61]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  The fragile nature of contextual preference reversals: Reply to Tsetsos, Chater, and Usher (2015). , 2015, Psychological review.

[62]  Nick Chater,et al.  Examining the mechanisms underlying contextual preference reversal: Comment on Trueblood, Brown, and Heathcote (2014). , 2015, Psychological review.

[63]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Decision by sampling , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[64]  Michael J Beran,et al.  Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) exhibit the decoy effect in a perceptual discrimination task , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[65]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The hare and the tortoise: emphasizing speed can change the evidence used to make decisions. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[66]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  A method for efficiently sampling from distributions with correlated dimensions. , 2013, Psychological methods.

[67]  H Pashler,et al.  How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing. , 2000, Psychological review.

[68]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  Likelihood-Free Methods for Cognitive Science , 2018 .

[69]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Multialternative decision field theory: a dynamic connectionist model of decision making. , 2001, Psychological review.

[70]  R. Luce,et al.  Individual Choice Behavior: A Theoretical Analysis. , 1960 .

[71]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  The Lognormal Race: A Cognitive-Process Model of Choice and Latency with Desirable Psychometric Properties , 2015, Psychometrika.

[72]  Han L J van der Maas,et al.  Optimal decision making in neural inhibition models. , 2012, Psychological review.

[73]  Dan R. Schley,et al.  Proleader and antitrailer information distortion and their effects on choice and postchoice memory , 2014 .

[74]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Product familiarity and learning new information , 1984 .

[75]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Drawing conclusions from choice response time models: A tutorial using the linear ballistic accumulator , 2011 .

[76]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  Approaches to Analysis in Model-based Cognitive Neuroscience. , 2017, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[77]  Pierre Chandon,et al.  Supersize in One Dimension, Downsize in Three Dimensions: Effects of Spatial Dimensionality on Size Perceptions and Preferences , 2009 .

[78]  N. Chater,et al.  Preference reversal in multiattribute choice. , 2010, Psychological review.

[79]  Michael D. Lee,et al.  A Survey of Model Evaluation Approaches With a Tutorial on Hierarchical Bayesian Methods , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[80]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[81]  J. Rieskamp,et al.  Rigorously testing multialternative decision field theory against random utility models. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[82]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  The dynamics of multimodal integration: The averaging diffusion model , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[83]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  The appropriacy of averaging in the study of context effects , 2016, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[84]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  Likelihood-free Bayesian analysis of memory models. , 2013, Psychological review.

[85]  M. Stone Models for choice-reaction time , 1960 .

[86]  Sudeep Bhatia,et al.  Associations and the accumulation of preference. , 2013, Psychological review.

[87]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Not Just for Consumers , 2013, Psychological science.

[88]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  The multiattribute linear ballistic accumulator model of context effects in multialternative choice. , 2014, Psychological review.

[89]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS Likelihood-free Bayesian Analysis of Memory Models , 2013 .

[90]  A. Tversky,et al.  Choice in Context: Tradeoff Contrast and Extremeness Aversion , 1992 .

[91]  T. Griffiths,et al.  Modeling individual differences using Dirichlet processes , 2006 .

[92]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The influence of urgency on decision time , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[93]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Bayesian analysis of simulation-based models , 2016 .

[94]  Jessica M. Choplin,et al.  Comparison-induced decoy effects , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[95]  Marius Usher,et al.  Disentangling decision models: from independence to competition. , 2013, Psychological review.

[96]  Nick Chater,et al.  Salience driven value integration explains decision biases and preference reversal , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.