On the Number of Neurons and Time Scale of Integration Underlying the Formation of Percepts in the Brain

All of our perceptual experiences arise from the activity of neural populations. Here we study the formation of such percepts under the assumption that they emerge from a linear readout, i.e., a weighted sum of the neurons’ firing rates. We show that this assumption constrains the trial-to-trial covariance structure of neural activities and animal behavior. The predicted covariance structure depends on the readout parameters, and in particular on the temporal integration window w and typical number of neurons K used in the formation of the percept. Using these predictions, we show how to infer the readout parameters from joint measurements of a subject’s behavior and neural activities. We consider three such scenarios: (1) recordings from the complete neural population, (2) recordings of neuronal sub-ensembles whose size exceeds K, and (3) recordings of neuronal sub-ensembles that are smaller than K. Using theoretical arguments and artificially generated data, we show that the first two scenarios allow us to recover the typical spatial and temporal scales of the readout. In the third scenario, we show that the readout parameters can only be recovered by making additional assumptions about the structure of the full population activity. Our work provides the first thorough interpretation of (feed-forward) percept formation from a population of sensory neurons. We discuss applications to experimental recordings in classic sensory decision-making tasks, which will hopefully provide new insights into the nature of perceptual integration.

[1]  David C. Hoyle,et al.  Accuracy of Pseudo-Inverse Covariance Learning—A Random Matrix Theory Analysis , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.

[2]  Romain Brette,et al.  Neuroinformatics Original Research Article Brian: a Simulator for Spiking Neural Networks in Python , 2022 .

[3]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[4]  B. Cumming,et al.  Decision-related activity in sensory neurons reflects more than a neuron’s causal effect , 2009, Nature.

[5]  Ulrike Goldschmidt,et al.  An Introduction To The Theory Of Point Processes , 2016 .

[6]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Dynamics of neuronal responses in macaque MT and VIP during motion detection , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  Naoshige Uchida,et al.  Odor Representations in Olfactory Cortex: Distributed Rate Coding and Decorrelated Population Activity , 2012, Neuron.

[8]  Ranulfo Romo,et al.  Linear readout from a neural population with partial correlation data , 2010, NIPS.

[9]  R. Born,et al.  Timescales of Sensory- and Decision-Related Activity in the Middle Temporal and Medial Superior Temporal Areas , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Movshon,et al.  A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  Radford M. Neal Pattern Recognition and Machine Learning , 2007, Technometrics.

[12]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[13]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[14]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Contribution of Middle Temporal Area to Coarse Depth Discrimination: Comparison of Neuronal and Psychophysical Sensitivity , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  R. Romo,et al.  Neural codes for perceptual discrimination in primary somatosensory cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  NEURAL ACTIVITY IN MECHANORECEPTIVE CUTANEOUS AFFERENTS: STIMULUS-RESPONSE RELATIONS, WEBER FUNCTIONS, AND INFORMATION TRANSMISSION. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Daryl J. Daley,et al.  An Introduction to the Theory of Point Processes , 2013 .

[18]  Eric R. Ziegel,et al.  The Elements of Statistical Learning , 2003, Technometrics.

[19]  A. Pouget,et al.  Neural correlations, population coding and computation , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  M K Habib,et al.  Dynamics of neuronal firing correlation: modulation of "effective connectivity". , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Robert P. W. Duin,et al.  Expected classification error of the Fisher linear classifier with pseudo-inverse covariance matrix , 1998, Pattern Recognit. Lett..

[22]  Y. Loewenstein,et al.  Bayesian Inference Underlies the Contraction Bias in Delayed Comparison Tasks , 2011, PloS one.

[23]  Peter Dayan,et al.  The Effect of Correlated Variability on the Accuracy of a Population Code , 1999, Neural Computation.

[24]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  The sense of flutter-vibration: comparison of the human capacity with response patterns of mechanoreceptive afferents from the monkey hand. , 1968, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  W. Newsome,et al.  Estimates of the Contribution of Single Neurons to Perception Depend on Timescale and Noise Correlation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  W. Bair,et al.  Correlated Firing in Macaque Visual Area MT: Time Scales and Relationship to Behavior , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Adam M. Packer,et al.  Inferring neural population dynamics from multiple partial recordings of the same neural circuit , 2013, NIPS.

[28]  Germán Sumbre,et al.  Fast functional imaging of multiple brain regions in intact zebrafish larvae using Selective Plane Illumination Microscopy , 2013, BMC Neuroscience.

[29]  Mark D. Humphries,et al.  Population-wide distributions of neural activity during perceptual decision-making , 2013, Progress in neurobiology.

[30]  E. Salinas,et al.  Perceptual decision making in less than 30 milliseconds , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  Christian K. Machens,et al.  Variability in neural activity and behavior , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[33]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal correlates of sensory discrimination in the somatosensory cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  M. Bethge,et al.  Inferring decoding strategies from choice probabilities in the presence of correlated variability , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  R. Romo,et al.  Frequency discrimination in the sense of flutter: psychophysical measurements correlated with postcentral events in behaving monkeys , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  W. Newsome,et al.  The Variable Discharge of Cortical Neurons: Implications for Connectivity, Computation, and Information Coding , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Philipp J. Keller,et al.  Whole-brain functional imaging at cellular resolution using light-sheet microscopy , 2013, Nature Methods.

[38]  R. Romo,et al.  Neural correlate of subjective sensory experience gradually builds up across cortical areas , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  M. Orger,et al.  Whole-Brain Activity Maps Reveal Stereotyped, Distributed Networks for Visuomotor Behavior , 2014, Neuron.

[40]  Christian K. Machens,et al.  Predictive Coding of Dynamical Variables in Balanced Spiking Networks , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[41]  Bruce Cumming,et al.  Correlations between the activity of sensory neurons and behavior: how much do they tell us about a neuron's causality? , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[42]  Simon R. Schultz,et al.  The Ising decoder: reading out the activity of large neural ensembles , 2010, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[43]  Sophie Denève,et al.  Spike-Based Population Coding and Working Memory , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..